SR Research
Fast, Accurate, Reliable Eye Tracking
lines

Full Publications List

print Print Page

A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z

A

Aamand, R., Dalsgaard, T., Ho, Y.-C. L., Møller, A., Roepstorff, A., & Lund, T. E. (2013). A NO way to BOLD?: Dietary nitrate alters the hemodynamic response to visual stimulation. NeuroImage, 83, 397-407.

Abegg, M., Lee H., & Barton J. J. S. (2010). Systematic diagonal and vertical errors in antisaccades and memory-guided saccades. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(3):5, 1-10.

Abegg, M., Manoach, D. S., Barton, J. J. S. (2011). Knowing the future: partial foreknowledge effects on the programming of prosaccades and antisaccades. Vision Research, 51, 215-221.

Abegg, M., Rodriguez, A. R., Lee, H., & Barton, J. J. S. (2010). 'Alternate-goal bias' in antisaccades and the influence of expectation. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 553-562.

Abegg, M., Sharma, N., & Barton, J. J. S. (2012). Antisaccades generate two types of saccadic inhibition. Biological Psychology, 89, 191-194.

Abekawa, N., & Gomi, H. (2010). Spatial coincidence of intentional actions modulates an implicit visuomotor control. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 2717-2727.

Abel, L. A., Wang, Z. I., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2008). Wavelet analysis in infantile nystagmus syndrome: Limitations and abilities. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 49, 3413-3423.

Ablinger, I., Huber, W., Schattka, K. I., & Radach, R. (2013). Recovery in a letter-by-letter reader: more efficiency at the expense of normal reading strategy. Neurocase, 19, 236-255.

Acha, J., & Perea, M. (2008). The effect of neighborhood frequency in reading: Evidence with transposed-letter neighbors. Cognition, 108, 290-300.

Acheson, D. J., & Hagoort, P. (2013). Stimulating the brain's language network: syntactic ambiguity resolution after TMS to the inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1664-1677.

Ackermann, J. F., & Landy, M. S. (2013). Choice of saccade endpoint under risk. Journal of Vision, 13(3):27, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/27, doi:10.1167/13.3.27.

Acunzo, D. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). No emotional "pop-out" effect in natural scene viewing. Emotion, 11, 1134-1143.

Adab, H. Z., & Vogels, R. (2011). Practicing coarse orientation discrimination improves orientation signals in Macaque cortical area V4. Current Biology, 21, 1661-1666.

Adam, J. J., Buetti, S., & Kerze, D. (2012). Coordinated flexibility: How initial gaze position modulates eye-hand coordination and reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 891-901.

Adam, R., Bays, P. M., & Husain, M. (2012). Rapid decision-making under risk. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 52-61.

Adam, R., Leff, A., Sinha, N., Turner, C., Bays, P., Draganski, B., & Husain, M. (2013). Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions. Cortex, 49, 1292-1303.

Afraz, A., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations. Journal of Vision, 9(10):10, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/9/10/10/, doi:10.1167/9.10.10.

Agrafiotis, D., Canagarajah, N., Bull, D. R., & Dye, M. (2003). Perceptually optimised sign language video coding based on eye tracking analysis. Electronics Letters, 39, 1703-1705.

Agrafiotis, D., Canagarajah, N., Bull, D. R., Kyle, J., Seers, H., & Dye, M. (2006). A perceptually optimised video coding system for sign language communication at low bit rates. Signal Processing: Image Communication, 21, 531-549.

Aguilar, C., & Castet, E. (2011). Gaze-contingent simulation of retinopathy: Some potential pitfalls and remedies. Vision Research, 51, 997-1012.

Ahken, S., Comeau, G., Hébert, S., & Balasubramaniam, R. (2012). Eye movement patterns during the processing of musical and linguistic syntactic incongruities. Psychomusicology, 22, 18-25.

Ahmadi, M., Judi, M., khorrami, A., hahmoudi-Gharaei, J., & Tehrani-Doost, M. (2011). Initial orientation of attention towards emotional faces in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 87-91.

Aichert, D. S., Derntl, B., Wöstmann, N. M., Groß, J. K., Dehning, S., Cerovecki, A., Möller, H.-J., Habel, U., Riedel, M., & Ettinger, U. (2013). Intact emotion-cognition interaction in schizophrenia patients and first-degree relatives: Evidence from an emotional antisaccade task. Brain and Cognition, 82, 329-336.

Aichert, D. S., Wöstmann, N. M., Costa, A., Macare, C., Wenig, J. R., Möller, H. -J., Rubia, K., & Ettinger, U. (2012). Associations between trait impulsivity and prepotent response inhibition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 1016-1032.

Aitkin, C.D., Santos, E.M., & Kowler, E. (2013). Anticipatory smooth eye movements in Autism Spectrum Disorder. PLoS ONE 8(12): e83230. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083230.

Åkerfelt, A., Colonius, H., & Diederich, A. (2006). Visual-tactile saccadic inhibition. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 554-63.

Akman, O. E., Clement, R. A., Broomhead, D. S., Mannan, S., Moorhead, I., & Wilson, H. R. (2009). Probing bottom-up processing with multistable images. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(3):4, 1-7.

Al-Aidroos, N., & Pratt, J. (2010). Top-down control in time and space: Evidence from saccadic latencies and trajectories. Visual Cognition, 18, 26-49.

Al-Aidroos, N., Fischer, M. H.. Adam, J. J., & Pratt, J. (2008). Structured perceptual arrays and the modulation of Fitts's Law: Examining saccadic eye movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40, 155-164.

Alahyane, N., & Pélisson, D. (2003). Adaptation of saccadic eye movements: Transfer and specificity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1004, 69-77.

Alahyane, N., & Pélisson, D. (2004). Eye position specificity of saccadic adaptation. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 45, 123-130.

Alahyane, N., & Pélisson, D. (2005). Long-lasting modifications of saccadic eye movements following adaptation induced in the double-step target paradigm. Learning and Memory, 12, 433-443.

Alahyane, N., & Pélisson, D. (2005). Retention of saccadic adaptation in humans. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 558-562.

Alahyane, N., Devauchelle, A.-D., Salemme, R., & Pélisson, D. (2008). Spatial transfer of adaptation of scanning voluntary saccades in humans. NeuroReport, 19, 37-41.

Alahyane, N., Fonteille, V., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Nighoghossian, N., Pelisson, D., & Tilikete, C. (2008). Separate neural substrates in the human cerebellum for sensory-motor adaptation of reactive and of scanning voluntary saccades. The Cerebellum, 7, 595-601.

Alahyane, N., Koene, A., & Pélisson, D. (2004). Transfer of adaptation from visually guided saccades to averaging saccades elicited by double visual targets. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 827-836.

Alahyane, N., Salemme, R., Urquizar, C., Cotti, J., Guillaume, A., Vercher, J.-L., & Pélisson, D. (2007). Oculomotor plasticity: Are mechanisms of adaptation for reactive and voluntary saccades separate? Brain Research, 1135, 107-121.

Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and Pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 287-299.

Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Chesnet, D., & Fayol, M. (2007). Parallel processing before and after pauses: A combined analysis of graphomotor and eye movements during procedural text production. In M. Torrance, L. v. Waes & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and Cognition: Research and Applications (pp.13-29). Amsterdam. Elsevier.

Alamargot, D., Plane, S., Lambert, E., & Chesnet, D. (2010). Using eye and pen movements to trace the development of writing expertise: case studies of a 7th, 9th and 12th grader, graduate student, and professional writer. Reading and Writing, 23, 853-888.

Alexander, R. G., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2011). Visual similarity effects in categorical search. Journal of Vision, 11(8):9, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/8/9, doi:10.1167/11.8.9.

Alexander, R. G., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2012). Effects of part-based similarity on visual search: The Frankenbear experiment. Vision Research, 54, 20-30.

Alink, A., Euler, F., Kriegeskorte, N., Singer, W., & Kohle, A. (2012). Auditory motion direction encoding in auditory cortex and high-level visual cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 969-978.

Allman, A. A., Ettinger, U., Joober, R., & O'Driscoll, G. A. (2012). Effects of methylphenidate on basic and higher-order oculomotor functions. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 26, 1471-1479.

Allman, A.-A., Benkelfat, C., Durand, F., Sibon, I., Dagher, A., Leyton, M., Baker, G. B., & O'Driscol, G. A. (2010). Effect of d-amphetamine on inhibition and motor planning as a function of baseline performance. Psychopharmacology, 211, 423-433.

Altmann, C. F., Deubelius, A., & Kourtzi, Z. (2004). Shape saliency modulates contextual processing in the human lateral occipital complex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 794-804.

Altmann, G. T .M., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 502-518.

Altmann, G. T. M. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: the 'blank screen paradigm'. Cognition, 93, B79-B87.

Altmann, G. T. M. (2011). Language can mediate eye movement control within 100 milliseconds, regardless of whether there is anything to move the eyes to. Acta Psychologica, 137, 190-200.

Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247-264.

Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2009). Discourse-mediation of the mapping between language and the visual world: Eye movements and mental representation. Cognition, 111, 55-71.

Amano, K., Takeda, T., Haji, T., Terao, M., Maruya, K., Matsumoto, K., Murakami, I., & Nishida, S. (2012). Human neural responses involved in spatial pooling of locally ambiguous motion signals. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 3493-3508.

Amemori, K., & Graybiel, A. M. (2012). Localized microstimulation of primate pregenual cingulate cortex induces negative decision-making. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 776-785.

Amlôt, R., & Walker, R. (2006). Are somatosensory saccades voluntary or reflexive? Experimental Brain Research, 168, 557-565.

Amlôt, R., Walker, R., Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2003). Multimodal visual-somatosensory integration in saccade generation. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1-15.

Andersen, G. J., Ni, R., Bian, Z., & Kang, J. (2011). Limits of spatial attention in three-dimensional space and dual-task driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 43, 381-390.

Anderson, B. A., & Yantis, S. (2012). Value-driven attentional and oculomotor capture during goal-directed, unconstrained viewing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1644-1653.

Anderson, B., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2008). Effects of temporal context and temporal expectancy on neural activity in inferior temporal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 46, 947-957.

Anderson, B., Mruczek, R. E. B., Kawasaki, K., & Sheinberg, D. (2008). Effects of familiarity on neural activity in monkey inferior temporal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2540-2552.

Anderson, E. J., Mannan, S. K., Husain, M., Rees, G., Sumner, P., Mort, D. J., McRobbie, D., & Kennard, C. (2007). Involvement of prefrontal cortex in visual search. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 289-302.

Anderson, E. J., Mannan, S. K., Rees, G., Sumner, P., & Kennard, C. (2008). A role for spatial and nonspatial working memory processes in visual search. Experimental Psychology, 5, 301-312.

Anderson, E. J., Mannan, S. K., Rees, G., Sumner, P., & Kennard, C. (2010). Overlapping functional anatomy for working memory and visual search. Experimental Brain Research, 200, 91-107.

Anderson, N. C., Bischof, W. F., Laidlaw, K. E. W., Risko, E. F., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Recurrence quantification analysis of eye movements. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 842-856.

Anderson, N. C., Risko, E. F., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Exploiting human sensitivity to gaze for tracking the eyes. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 843-852.

Andersson, R., Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). I see what you're saying: The integration of complex speech and scenes during language comprehension. Acta Psychologica, 137, 208-216.

Andrews, S., Miller, B., & Rayner, K. (2004). Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in mousetrap. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 285-311.

Angele, B., & Rayner, K. (2011). Parafoveal processing of word n + 2 during reading: Do the preceding words matter? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1210-1220.

Angele, B., & Rayner, K. (2013). Eye movements and parafoveal preview of compound words: Does morpheme order matter? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 505-526.

Angele, B., & Rayner, K. (2013). Processing the in the Parafovea: Are articles skipped automatically? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 39, 649-662.

Angele, B., Slattery, T., Yang, J., Kliegl, R., & Rayner, K. (2008). Parafoveal processing in reading: Manipulating n+1 and n+2 previews simultaneously. Visual Cognition, 16, 697-707.

Angele, B., Tran, R., & Rayner, K. (2013). Parafoveal-foveal overlap can facilitate ongoing word identification during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 526-538.

Ansorge, U., Priess, H.-W., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Effects of relevant and irrelevant color singletons on inhibition of return and attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1687-1702.

Anton-Erxleben, K., Herrmann, K., & Carrasco, M. (2013). Independent effects of adaptation and attention on perceived speed. Psychological Science, 24, 150-159.

Apel, J. K., Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (2012). Targeting regressions: Do readers pay attention to the left? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1108-1113.

Apel, J. K., Revie, G. F., Cangelosi, A., Ellis, R., Goslin, J., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Attention deployment during memorizing and executing complex instructions. Experimental Brain Research, 214, 249-259.

Apel, K., Brimo, D., Wilson-Fowler, E. B., Vorstius, C., & Radach, R. (2013). Children develop initial orthographic knowledge during storybook reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 286-302.

Apfelbaum, K. S., Blumstein, S. E., & McMurray, B. (2011). Semantic priming is affected by real-time phonological competition: Evidence for continuous cascading systems. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 141-149.

Arai, M., & Keller, F. (2013). The use of verb-specific information for prediction in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 525-560.

Arai, M., van Gompel, R. P. G., & Scheepers, C. (2007). Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 218-250.

Archibald, J. (2005). Second language phonology as redeployment of L1 phonological knowledge. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 50, 285-314.

Archibald, N. K., Hutton, S. B., Clarke, M. P., Mosimann, U. P., & Burn, D. J. (2013). Visual exploration in Parkinson's disease and Parkinson's disease dementia. Brain, 136, 739-750.

Arizpe, J., Kravitz, D. J., Yovel, G., & Baker, C. I. (2012). Start position strongly influences fixation patterns during face processing: Difficulties with eye movements as a measure of information Use. PLoS ONE 7(2): e31106. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031106.

Armstrong, I. T., & Munoz, D. P. (2003). Attentional blink in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Influence of eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 243-250.

Armstrong, I. T., & Munoz, D. P. (2003). Inhibitory control of eye movements during oculomotor countermanding in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 444-452.

Arnold, J. E. (2008). THE BACON not the bacon: How children and adults understand accented and unaccented noun phrases. Cognition, 108, 69-99.

Arnold, J. E., & Lao, S.-Y. C. (2008). Put in last position something previously unmentioned: Word order effects on referential expectancy and reference comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 282-295.

Arnold, J. E., Kam, C. L. H., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007). If you say thee uh - you're describing something hard: the on-line attribution of disfluency during reference comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 914-930.

Asaad, W. F., Santhanam, N., McClellan, S., & Freedman, D. J. (2013). High-performance execution of psychophysical tasks with complex visual stimuli in MATLAB. Journal of Neurophysiology, 109, 249-260.

Ashby, J., Dix, H., Bontrager, M., Dey, R., & Archer, A. (2013). Phonemic awareness contributes to text reading fluency: Evidence From Eye Movements. School Psychology Review, 42, 157-170.

Ashby, J., Yang, J., Evans, K. H. C., & Rayner, K. (2012). Eye movements and the perceptual span in silent and oral reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 634-640.

Asher, M. F.,Tolhurst, D. J.,Troscianko,T., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2013). Regional effects of clutter on human target detection. Journal of Vision, 13(5):25, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/5/25, doi:10.1167/13.5.25.

Ashida, H., Kuriki, I., Murakami, I., Hisakata, R., & Kitaoka, A. (2012). Direction-specific fMRI adaptation reveals the visual cortical network underlying the "Rotating Snakes" illusion. NeuroImage, 61, 1143-1152.

Au, R. K. C., Ono, F., & Watanabe, K. (2012). Time dilation induced by object motion is based on spatiotopic but not retinotopic positions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:58. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00058.

Au-Yeung, S. K., Benson, V., Castelhano, M., & Rayner, K. (2011). Eye Movement Sequences during Simple versus Complex Information Processing of Scenes in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Research and Treatment, vol. 2011, Article ID 657383, 7 pages, 2011. doi:10.1155/2011/657383.

Austin, A. J., & Duka, T. (2010). Mechanisms of attention for appetitive and aversive outcomes in Pavlovian conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research, 213, 19-26.

Austin, A. J., & Duka, T. (2012). Mechanisms of attention to conditioned stimuli predictive of a cigarette outcome. Behavioural Brain Research, 232, 183-189.

Authié, C. N., & Mestre, D. R. (2011). Optokinetic nystagmus is elicited by curvilinear optic flow during high speed curve driving. Vision Research, 51, 1791-1800.

Aviezer, H., Ran, H., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J. M., Anderson, A., Moscovitch, M., & Schlomo, B. (2008). Angry, disgusted or afraid? Studies on the malleability of facial expression perception. Psychological Science, 19, 724-732.

Aydin, S., Strang, N. C., & Manahilov, V. (2013). Age-related deficits in attentional control of perceptual rivalry. Vision Research, 77, 32-40.

Açik, A., Onat, S., Schumann, F., Einhäuser, W., & König, P. (2009). Effects of luminance contrast and its modifications on fixation behavior during free viewing of images from different categories. Vision Research,49, 1541-1553.

Açik, A., Sarwary, A., Schultze-Kraft, R., Onat, S., & König, P. (2010). Developmental changes in natural viewing behavior: bottom-up and top-down differences between children, young adults and older adults. Front. Psychology 1:207. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00207.

Back To Top

B

Backus, B. T., & Matza-Brown, D. (2003). The contribution of vergence change to the measurement of relative disparity. Journal of Vision, 3, 737-750.

Badham, S. P., & Hutchinson, C. V. (2013). Characterising eye movement dysfunction in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 251, 2769-2776.

Badler, J. B., Lefèvre, P., & Missal, M. (2008). Anticipatory pursuit is influenced by a concurrent timing task. Journal of Vision, 8(16):5, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/8/16/5/, doi:10.1167/8.16.5.

Badler, J., Lefèvre, P., & Missal, M. (2010). Causality attribution biases oculomotor responses. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 10517-10525.

Bai, X., Liang, F., Blythe, H. I., Zang, C., Yan, G., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Interword spacing effects on the acquisition of new vocabulary for readers of Chinese as a second language. Journal of Research in Reading, 36, S4-S17.

Bai, X., Yan, G., Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C., & Rayner, K. (2008). Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1277-1287.

Baker, D. H., & Graf, E. W. (2010). Extrinsic factors in the perception of bistable motion stimuli. Vision Research, 50, 1257-1265.

Baker, D.A., Schweitzer. N.J., Risko. E.F., & Ware, J.M. (2013). Visual attention and the neuroimage bias. PLoS ONE 8(9): e74449. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074449.

Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2008). Visual attention during the preparation of bimanual movements. Vision Research, 48, 549-563.

Baldauf, D., Wolf, M., & Deubel, H. (2006). Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements. Vision Research, 46, 4355-4374.

Balslev, D., Odoj, B., & Karnath, H.-O. (2013). Role of somatosensory cortex in visuospatial attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 18311-18318.

Banerjee, S., Snyder, A. C., Molholm, S., & Foxe, J. J. (2011). Oscillatory Alpha-band mechanisms and the deployment of spatial attention to anticipated auditory and visual target locations: Supramodal or sensory-specific control mechanisms? Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9923-9932.

Baptista, M. S., Bohn, C., Kliegl, R., Engbert, R., & Kurths, J. (2008). Reconstruction of eye movements during blinks. Chaos, 18, 013126.

Barense, M. D., Groen, I. I. A., Lee, A. C. H., Yeung, L.-K., Brady, S. M., Gregori, M., Kapur, N., Bussey, T. J., Saksida, L. M., & Henson, R. N. A. (2012). Intact memory for irrelevant information impairs perception in amnesia. Neuron, 75, 157-167.

Barot, N., McLean, R. J., Gottlob, I., & Proudlock, F. A. (2013). Reading Performance in Infantile Nystagmus. Ophthalmology, 120, 1232-1238.

Barr, D. J. (2008). Pragmatic expectations and linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground. Cognition, 109, 18-40.

Barr, D. J., & Keysar, B. (2002). Anchoring comprehension in linguistic precedents. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 391-418.

Barrington, L., Marks, T. K., Hsiao, J. H., & Cottrell, G. W. (2008). NIMBLE: A kernel density model of saccade-based visual memory. Journal of Vision, 8(14):17, 1-14, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/17/, doi:10.1167/8.14.17.

Bartek, B., Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Smith, M. R. (2011). In search of on-line locality effects in sentence comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1178-1198.

Bartels, M., & Marshall, S. P. (2011). Eye tracking and universal access: Three applications and practical examples. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 6766, 525-534.

Bate, S., Haslam, C., & Hodgson, T. L. (2009). Angry faces are special too: Evidence from the visual scanpath. Neuropsychology, 23, 658-667.

Bate, S., Haslam, C., Hodgson, T. L., Jansari, A., Gregory, N., & Kay, J. (2010). Positive and negative emotion enhances the processing of famous faces in a semantic judgment task. Neuropsychology, 24, 84-89.

Bate, S., Haslam, C., Jansari, A., & Hodgson, T. L. (2009). Covert face recognition relies on affective valence in congenital prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 391-411.

Bate, S., Haslam, C., Tree, J. J., & Hodgson, T. L., (2008). Evidence of an eye movement-based memory effect in congenital Prosopagnosia. Cortex, 44, 806-819.

Baudiffier, V., Caplan, D., Gaonac'h, D., & Chesnet, D. (2011). The effect of noun animacy on the processing of unambiguous sentences: Evidence from French relative clauses. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1896-1905.

Bauer, M., Akam, T., Joseph, S., Freeman, E., & Driver, J. (2012). Does visual flicker phase at gamma frequency modulate neural signal propagation and stimulus selection? Journal of Vision, 12(4):5, 1-10, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/4/5, doi:10.1167/12.4.5.

Baumann, O., & Mattingley, J. B. (2010). Scaling of neural responses to visual and auditory motion in the human cerebellum. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4489-4495.

Baumann, O., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Functional topography of primary emotion processing in the human cerebellum. NeuroImage, 61, 805-811.

Bayless, S. J., Glover, M., Taylor, M .J., & Itier, R. J. (2011). Is it in the eyes? Dissociating the role of emotion and perceptual features of emotionally expressive faces in modulating orienting to eye gaze. Visual Cognition, 19, 483-510.

Bayliss, A. P., Murphy, E., Naughtin, C. K., Kritikos, A., Schilbach, L., & Becker, S. I. (2013). "Gaze leading": Initiating simulated joint attention influences eye movements and choice behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 76-92.

Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2004). Orienting of attention via observed eye gaze is head-centred. Cognition, 94, B1-B10.

Bays, P. M., Catalao, R. F. G., & Husain, M. (2009). The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource. Journal of Vision, 9(10):7, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/9/10/7/, doi:10.1167/9.10.7.

Bays, P. M., Singh-Curry, V., Gorgoraptis, N., Driver, J., & Husain, M. (2010). Integration of goal- and stimulus-related visual signals revealed by damage to human parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 5968-5978.

Bays, P. M., Wu, E. Y., & Husain, M. (2011). Storage and binding of object features in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1622-1631.

Bebko, G. M., Franconeri, S. L., Ochsner, K. N., & Chiao, J. Y. (2011). Look before you regulate: Differential perceptual strategies underlying expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Emotion, 11, 732-742.

Becic, E., Boot, W. R., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Training older adults to search more effectively: Scanning strategy and visual search in dynamic displays. Psychology and Aging, 23, 461-466.

Becic, E., Kramer, A. F., & Boot, W. R. (2007). Age-related differences in the use of background layout in visual search. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 109-125.

Beck, M. R., Lohrenz, M. C., & Trafton, J. G. (2010). Measuring search efficiency in complex visual search tasks: Global and local clutter. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 238-250.

Beck, M. R., Peterson, M. S., Boot, W. R., Vomela, M., & Kramer, A. F. (2006). Explicit memory for rejected distractors during visual search. Visual Cognition, 14, 150-174.

Beck, M. R., Peterson, M. S., Vomela, M. (2006). Memory for where, but not what, is used during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 235-250.

Beck, V. M., Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2012). Simultaneous control of attention by multiple working memory representations. Psychological Science, 23, 887-898.

Becker, M. W., & Detweiler-Bedell, B. (2009). Early detection and avoidance of threatening faces during passive viewing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1257-1264.

Becker, M. W., & Rasmussen, I. P. (2007). The rhythm aftereffect: Support for time sensitive neurons with broad overlapping tuning curves. Brain and Cognition, 64, 274-281.

Becker, M. W., & Rasmussen, I. P. (2008). Guidance of attention to objects and locations by long-term memory of natural scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1325-1338.

Becker, S. I. (2010). Oculomotor Capture by colour singletons depends on intertrial priming. Vision Research, 50, 2116-2126.

Becker, S. I. (2010). Testing a postselectional account of across-dimension switch costs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 853-861.

Becker, S. I. (2010). The role of target-distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 247-265.

Becker, S. I. (2011). Determinants of dwell time in visual search: Similarity or perceptual difficulty? PLoS ONE 6(3): e17740. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.001774.

Becker, S. I. (2013). Simply shapely: Relative, not absolute shapes are primed in pop-out search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 845-861.

Becker, S. I., & Ansorge, U. (2013). Higher set sizes in pop-out search displays do not eliminate priming or enhance target selection. Vision Research, 81, 18-28.

Becker, S. I., Ansorge, U., & Turatto, M. (2009). Saccades reveal that allocentric coding of the moving object causes mislocalization in the flash-lag effect. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 1313-1324.

Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2010). The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1460-1476.

Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24, 634-647.

Becker, S. I., Horstmann, G., & Remington, R. W. (2011). Perceptual grouping, not emotion, accounts for search asymmetries with schematic faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1739-1757.

Beddor, P.S., McGowan, K.B., Boland, J.E., Coetzee, A.W., & Brasher, A. (2013). The time course of perception of coarticulation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133, 2350-2366.

Beedie, S. A., Benson, P. J., Giegling, I., Rujescu, D., & St. Clair, D. M. (2012). Smooth pursuit and visual scanpaths: Independence of two candidate oculomotor risk markers for schizophrenia. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 13, 200-210.

Beintema, J. A., van Loon, E. M., & van den Berg, A. V. (2005). Manipulating saccadic decision-rate distributions in visual search. Journal of Vision, 5, 150-164.

Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370-374.

Bekkering, H., Neggers, S. F. W., Walker, R., Gleissner, B., Dittrich, W. H., & Kennard, C. (2001). The preparation and execution of saccadic eye and goal-directed hand movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 39, 173-183.

Belke, E. (2006). Visual determinants of preferred adjective order. Visual Cognition, 14, 261-294.

Belke, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2002). Tracking the time course of multidimensional stimulus discrimination: Analyses of viewing patterns and processing times during 'same'-'different' decisions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 237-266.

Belke, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2007). Single and multiple object naming in healthy ageing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 1178-1211.

Belke, E., Humphreys, G. W., Watson, D. G., Meyer, A. S., & Telling, A. L. (2008). Top-down effects of semantic knowledge in visual search are modulated by cognitive but not perceptual load. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1444-1458.

Belke, E., Meyer, A. S., & Damian, M. F. (2005). Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 667-692.

Bellebaum, C., Daum, I., Koch, B., Schwarz, M., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (2005). The role of the human thalamus in processing corollary discharge. Brain, 128, 1139-1154.

Bellebaum, C., Hoffmann, K.-P., & Daum, I. (2005). Post-saccadic updating of visual space in the posterior parietal cortex in humans. Behavioural Brain Research, 163, 194-203.

Bellmann, C., Feely, M., Crossland, M. D., Kabanarou, S. A., & Rubin, G. S. (2004). Fixation stability using central and pericentral fixation targets in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Ophthalmology, 111, 2265-2270.

Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). Inhibition of saccadic eye movements to locations in spatial working memory. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 620-631.

Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). No functional role of attention-based rehearsal in maintenance of spatial working memory representations. Acta Psychologica, 132, 124-135.

Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). When are attention and saccade preparation dissociated? Psychological Science, 20, 1340-1347.

Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). Selection within visual memory representations activates the oculomotor system. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1605-1610.

Belopolsky, A. V., Devue, C., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). Angry faces hold the eyes. Visual Cognition, 19, 27-36.

Belyusar, D., Snyder, A. C., Frey, H.-P., Harwood, M. R., Wallman, J., & Foxe, J. J. (2013). Oscillatory alpha-band suppression mechanisms during the rapid attentional shifts required to perform an anti-saccade task. NeuroImage, 65, 395-407.

Ben-David, B. M., Chambers, C., Daneman, M., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Reingold, E., & Schneider, B. A. (2011). Effects of aging and noise on real-time spoken word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 243-262.

Benatar, A., & Clifton, C. Jr. (2014). Newness, givenness and discourse updating: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 1-16.

Bender, J., Tark, K.-J., Reuter, B., Kathmann, N., & Curtis, C. E. (2013). Differential roles of the frontal and parietal cortices in the control of saccades. Brain and Cognition, 83, 1-9.

Benjamins, J. S., Hooge, I. T. C., van Elst, J. C., Wertheim, A. H., & Verstraten, F. A. J. (2009). Search time critically depends on irrelevant subset size in visual search. Vision Research, 49, 398-406.

Benmussa, F., Aissani, C., Paradis, A.-L., & Lorenceau, J. (2011). Coupled dynamics of bistable distant motion displays. Journal of Vision, 11(8):14, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/8/14, doi:10.1167/11.8.14.

Benson, P. J., Beedie, S. A., Shephard, E., Giegling, I., Rujescu, D., & St Clair, D. (2012). Simple viewing tests can detect eye movement abnormalities that distinguish schizophrenia cases from controls with exceptional accuracy. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 716-724.

Benson, P. J., Leonards, U., Lothian, R. M., St. Clair, D. M., & Merlo, M. C. G. (2007). Visual scan paths in first-episode schizophrenia and cannabis-induced psychosis. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 32, 267-274.

Benson, V., Castelhano, M. S., Au-Yeung, S. K. , & Rayner, K. (2012). Eye movements reveal no immediate ‘WOW’ effect in Autism Spectrum Disorder. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1139-1150.

Benson, V., Ietswaart, M., & Milner, D. (2012). Eye movements and verbal report in a single case of visual neglect. PLoS ONE 7(8): e43743. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043743.

Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 3253 - 3258.

Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2013). The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition, 127, 391-397.

Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2013). Young toddlers' word comprehension is flexible and efficient. PLoS ONE 8(8): e73359. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073359.

Bergert, S. (2013). How do our brain hemispheres cooperate to avoid false memories? Cortex, 49, 572-581.

Bergert, S., Windmann, S., & Güntürkün, O. (2006). Is interhemispheric communication disturbed when the two hemispheres perform on separate tasks? Neuropsychologia, 44, 1457-1467.

Berggren, N., Hutton, S. B., & Derakshan, N. (2011). The effects of self-report cognitive failures and cognitive load on antisaccade performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:280. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00280.

Berggren, N., Richards, A., Taylor, J., & Derakshan, N. (2013). Affective attention under cognitive load: reduced emotional biases but emergent anxiety-related costs to inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:188. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00188.

Bergsma, D. P., & van der Wildt, G. (2010). Visual training of cerebral blindness patients gradually enlarges the visual field. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 94, 88-96.

Bergsma, D. P., Elshout, J. A., van der Wildt, G. J., & van den Berg, A. A. (2012). Transfer effects of training-induced visual field recovery in patients with chronic stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 19, 212-225.

Bernard, J.-B., Kumar, G., Junge, J., & Chung, S. T. L. (2013). The effect of letter-stroke boldness on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 84, 33-42.

Bernard, J.-B., Scherlen, A.-C., & Castet, E. (2007). Page mode reading with simulated scotomas: A modest effect of interline spacing on reading speed. Vision Research, 47, 3447-3459.

Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2003). The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory & Language, 48, 615-634.

Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2013). The role of hyphens at the constituent boundary in compound word identification. Experimental Psychology, 60, 157-167.

Bertram, R., Helle, L., Kaakinen, J.K., & Svedström, E. (2013). The effect of expertise on eye movement behaviour in medical image perception. PLoS ONE 8(6): e66169. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0066169.

Bertram, R., Hyönä, J., & Laine, M. (2000). The role of context in morphological processing: Evidence from Finnish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 367-388.

Bertram, R., Kuperman, V., Baayen, R. H., & Hyönä, J. (2011). The hyphen as a segmentation cue in triconstituent compound processing: It’s getting better all the time. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 52, 530-544.

Bertram, R., Pollatsek, A., & Hyönä, J. (2004). Morphological parsing and the use of segmentation cues in reading Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 325-345.

Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Tatler, B. W., Phillips, L. H., Fraser, G., Benson, P. J., & St.Clair, D. (2006). Global visual scanning abnormalities in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 87, 212-222.

Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2006). Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades. NeuroReport, 17, 1001-1004.

Betta, E., Galfano, G., & Turatto, M. (2007). Microsaccadic response during inhibition of return in a target-target paradigm. Vision Research, 47, 428-436.

Bettenbühl, M., Paladini, C., Mergenthaler, K., Kliegl, R., Engbert, R., & Holschneider, M. (2010). Microsaccade characterization using the continuous wavelet transform and principal component analysis. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(5):1, 1-14.

Bettenbühl, M., Rusconi, M., Engbert, R., & Holschneider, M. (2012) Bayesian selection of Markov Models for symbol sequences: Application to microsaccadic eye movements. PLoS ONE 7(9): e43388. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043388.

Betts, L. R., Sekuler, A. B., & Bennett, P. J. (2007). The effects of aging on orientation discrimination. Vision Research, 47, 1769-1780.

Betts, L. R., Taylor, C. P., Sekuler, A. B., & Bennett, P. J. (2005). Aging reduces center-surround antagonism in visual motion processing. Neuron, 45, 361-366.

Betz, T., Kietzmann, T. C., Wilming, N., & König, P. (2010). Investigating task-dependent top-down effects on overt visual attention. Journal of Vision, 10(3):15, 1-14, http://journalofvision.org/10/3/15/, doi:10.1167/10.3.15.

Betz, T., Wilming, N., Bogler, C., Haynes, J.-D., & Konig, P. (2013). Dissociation between saliency signals and activity in early visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 13(14):6, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/14/6, doi:10.1167/13.14.6.

Beurskens, R., & Bock, O. (2012). Age-related decline of peripheral visual processing: the role of eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 217, 117-124.

Beurze, S. M., van Pelt, S., & Medendorp, W. P. (2006). Behavioral reference frames for planning human reaching movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 352-362.

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of Aging and Bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 1341-1354.

Bibi, R., & Edelman, J. A. (2009). The influence of motor training on human express saccade production. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 3101-3110.

Bichot, N. P., Heard, M. T., & Desimone, R. (2011). Stimulation of the nucleus accumbens as behavioral reward in awake behaving monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 199, 265-272.

Bieg, H.-J., Bresciani, J.-P., Bülthoff, H. H., & Chuang, L. L. (2012) Looking for discriminating is different from looking for looking's sake. PLoS ONE 7(9): e45445. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045445.

Bieg, H.-J., Bresciani, J.-P., Bülthoff, H. H., & Chuang, L. L. (2013). Saccade reaction time asymmetries during task-switching in pursuit tracking. Experimental Brain Research, 20, 271-281.

Bieg, H.-J., Bülthoff, H. H., & Chuang, L. L. (2013). Attentional biases during steering behavior. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8025, 21-27.

Biggs, A. T., Brockmole, J. R., & Witt, J. K. (2013). Armed and attentive: Holding a weapon can bias attentional priorities in scene viewing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1715-1724.

Bindemann, M. (2010). Scene and screen center bias early eye movements in scene viewing. Vision Research, 50, 2577-2587.

Bindemann, M., & Lewis, M. B. (2013). Face detection differs from categorization: Evidence from visual search in natural scenes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1140-1145.

Bindemann, M., Sandford, A., Gillatt, K., Avetisyan, M., & Megreya, A. M. (2012). Recognising faces seen alone or with others: Why are two heads worse than one? Perception, 41, 415-435.

Bindemann, M., Scheepers, C., & Burton, A. M. (2009). Viewpoint and center of gravity affect eye movements to human faces. Journal of Vision, 9(2):7, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/9/2/7/, doi:10.1167/9.2.7.

Bindemann, M., Scheepers, C., Ferguson, H. J., & Burton, A. M. (2010). Face, body, and center of gravity mediate person detection in natural scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1477-1485.

Birch, E. E., Wang, J., Felius, J., Stager, D. R., Jr., & Hertle, R. W. (2012). Fixation control and eye alignment in children treated for dense congenital or developmental cataracts. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 16, 156-160.

Bird, G. D., Lauwereyns, J., & Crawford, M. T. (2012). The role of eye movements in decision making and the prospect of exposure effects. Vision Research, 60, 16-21.

Bird, G., Catmur, C., Silani, G., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 31, 1614-1624.

Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2007). Why do we look at people's eyes? Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(1):1, 1-6, http://jemr.org/.

Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Gaze selection in complex social scenes. Visual Cognition, 16, 341-355.

Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Social attention and real-world scenes: The roles of action, competition and social content. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 986-998.

Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Get real! Resolving the debate about equivalent social stimuli. Visual Cognition, 17, 904-924.

Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Saliency does not account for fixations to eyes within social scenes. Vision Research, 49, 2992-3000.

Birmingham, E., Cerf, M., & Adolphs, R. (2011). Comparing social attention in autism and amygdala lesions: Effects of stimulus and task condition. Social Neuroscience, 6, 420-435.

Bissett, P. G., & Logan, G. D. (2013). Stop before you leap: Changing eye and hand movements requires stopping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 941-946.

Blais, C., Jack, R. E., Scheepers, C, Fiset, D., & Caldara, R. (2008). Culture shapes how we look at faces. PLoS ONE. 2008; 3(8): e3022.

Blakely, D. P., Wright, T., Dehili, V. M., Boot, W. R., & Brockmole, J. R. (2012). Characterizing the time course and nature of attentional disengagement effects. Vision Research, 56, 38-48.

Blanchard, T.C., Pearson, J.M., & Hayden, B. Y. (2013). Postreward delays and systematic biases in measures of animal temporal discounting. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 15491-15496;

Blanchard, T.C., Wolfe, L. S., Vlaev, I., Winston, J. S., & Hayden, B. Y. (2014). Biases in preferences for sequences of outcomes in monkeys. Cognition, 130, 289-299.

Blanke, M., Harsch, L., Knöll, J., & Bremmer, F. (2010). Spatial perception during pursuit initiation. Vision Research, 50, 2714-2720.

Blaukopf, C. L., & DiGirolamo, G. J. (2005). The automatic extraction and use of information from cues and go signals in an antisaccade task. Experimental Brain Research, 167, 654-659.

Blaukopf, C. L., & DiGirolamo, G. J. (2006). Differential effects of reward and punishment on conscious and unconscious eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 174, 786-792.

Blechert, J., Ansorge, U., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2010). A body-related dot-probe task reveals distinct attentional patterns for Bulimia Nervosa and Anorexia Nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 575-585.

Bledowski, C., Rahm, B., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). What "works" in working memory? Separate systems for selection and updating of critical information. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 13735-13741.

Blekher, T. M., Weaver, M. R., Cai, X., Hui, S., Marshall, J. D., Jackson, J. M., Wojcieszek, J. M., Yee, R. D., & Foroud, T. M. (2009). Test-retest reliability of saccadic measures in subjects at-risk for Huntington disease. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 50, 5707-5711.

Blekher, T. M., Yee, R. D., Kirkwood, S. C., Hake, A. M., Stout, J. C., Weaver, M. R., & Foroud, T. M. (2004). Oculomotor control in asymptomatic and recently diagnosed individuals with the genetic marker for Huntington's disease. Vision Research, 44, 2729-2736.

Blekher, T., Johnson, S. A., Marshall, J., White, K., Hui, S., Weaver, M., Gray, J., Yee, R., Stout, J. C., Beristain, X., Wojcieszek, J., & Foroud, T. (2006). Saccades in presymptomatic and early stages of Huntington disease. Neurology, 67, 394-399.

Blekher, T., Weaver, M. R., Marshall, J., Hui, S., Jackson, J. G., Stout, J. C., Beristain, X., Wojcieszek, J., Yee, R. D., Foroud, T. M. (2009). Visual scanning and cognitive performance in prediagnostic and early-stage Huntington's disease. Movement Disorders, 24, 532-538.

Blekher, T., Weaver, M., Rupp, J., Nichols, W. C., Hui, S. L., Gray, J., Yee, R. D., Wojcieszek, J., & Foroud, T. (2009). Multiple step pattern as a biomarker in Parkinson disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 15, 506-510.

Bleumers, L., De Graef, P., Verfaillie, K., & Wagemans, J. (2008). Eccentric grouping by proximity in multistable dot lattices. Vision Research, 48, 179-192.

Blythe, H. I., Häikiö, T., Bertam, R., Liversedge, S. P., & Hyönä, J. (2011). Reading disappearing text: Why do children refixate words? Vision Research, 51, 84-92.

Blythe, H. I., Liang, F., Zang, C., Wang, J., Yan, G., Bai, X., & Liversedge, S. P. (2012). Inserting spaces into Chinese text helps readers to learn new words: An eye movement study. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 241-254.

Bock, K., & Irwin, D. E., Davidson, D. J., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2003). Minding the clock. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 653-685.

Boehler, C. N., Hopf, J.-M., Krebs, R. M., Stoppel, C. M., Schoenfeld, M. A., Heinze, H.-J., & Noesselt, T. (2011). Task-load-dependent activation of dopaminergic midbrain areas in the absence of reward. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4955-4961.

Bogartz, R. S., & Staub, A. (2012). Gaze step distributions reflect fixations and saccades: A comment on Stephen and Mirman (2012). Cognition,

Bojko, A., Kramer, A. F., & Peterson, M. S. (2004). Age equivalence in switch costs for prosaccade and antisaccade tasks. Psychology and Aging, 19, 226-234.

Bolger, P. & Zapata, G. (2011). Semantic categories and context in L2 vocabulary learning. Language Learning, 61, 614-646.

Bond, G. D. (2008). Deception detection expertise. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 339-351.

Bonev, B., Chuang, L. L., & Escolano, F. (2013). How do image complexity, task demands and looking biases influence human gaze behavior? Pattern Recognition Letters, 34, 723-730.

Bonitz, V. S., & Gordon, R. D. (2008). Attention to smoking-related and incongruous objects during scene viewing. Acta Psychologica, 129, 255-263.

Bonneh, Y. S., Donner, T. H., Sagi, D., Fried, M., Cooperman, A., Heeger, D. J., & Arieli, A. (2010). Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: Cause and effect. Journal of Vision, 10(14):22, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/22, doi:10.1167/10.14.22.

Bonnetblanc, F., & Baraduc, P. (2007). Saccadic adaptation without retinal postsaccadic error. NeuroReport, 18, 1399-1402.

Boon, P. J., Theeuwes, J., & Belopolsky, A. V. (2014). Updating visual-spatial working memory during object movement. Vision Research, 94, 51-57.

Boot, W. R., & Brockmole, J. R. (2010). Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search. Visual Cognition, 18, 481-491.

Boot, W. R., Becic, E., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Stable individual differences in search strategy?: The effect of task demands and motivational factors on scanning strategy in visual search. Journal of Vision, 9(3):7, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/9/3/7/, doi:10.1167/9.3.7.

Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., & Peterson, M. S. (2005). Oculomotor consequences of abrupt object onsets and offsets: Onsets dominate oculomotor capture. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 910-928.

Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Becic, E., Wiegmann, A. A., & Kubose, T. (2006). Detecting transient changes in dynamic displays: The more you look, the less you see. Human Factors, 48, 759-773.

Boot, W. R., McCarley, J. S, Kramer, A. F., & Peterson, M. S. (2004). Automatic and intentional memory processes in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 854-861.

Boot, W. R., Neider, M. B., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Training and transfer of training in the search for camouflaged targets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 950-963.

Booth, R. W., & Weger, U. W. (2013). The function of regressions in reading: Backward eye movements allow rereading. Memory & Cognition, 41, 82-97.

Born, S., Ansorge, U., & Kerzel, D. (2012). Feature-based effects in the coupling between attention and saccades. Journal of Vision, 12(11):27, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/11/27, doi:10.1167/12.11.27.

Born, S., Ansorge, U., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Predictability of spatial and non-spatial target properties improves perception in the pre-saccadic interval. Vision Research, 91, 93-101.

Born, S., Kerzel, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). Evidence for a dissociation between the control of oculomotor capture and disengagement. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 621-631.

Borovsky, A., Burns, E., Elman, J. L., & Evans, J. L. (2013). Lexical activation during sentence comprehension in adolescents with history of Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46, 413-427.

Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one's age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 417-436.

Bosch, S. E., Neggers, S. F. W., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2013). The role of the frontal eye fields in oculomotor competition: Image-guided TMS enhances contralateral target selection. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 824-832.

Bosco, G., Delle Monache, S., & Lacquaniti, F. (2012). Catching what we can't see: Manual interception of occluded fly-ball trajectories. PLoS ONE 7(11): e49381. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049381.

Boström, K. J., & Warzecha, A.-K. (2009). Ocular following response to sampled motion. Vision Research, 49, 1693-1701.

Boström, K. J., & Warzecha, A.-K. (2010). Open-loop speed discrimination performance of ocular following response and perception. Vision Research, 50, 870-882.

Bott, O. (2013). The processing domain of aspectual interpretation. Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy, 93, 195-229.

Bottari, D., Valsecchi, M., & Pavani, F. (2012). Prominent reflexive eye-movement orienting associated with deafness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 8-13.

Boucher, L., Stuphorn, V., Logan, G. D., Schall, J. D., & Palmeri, T. J. (2007). Stopping eye and hand movements: Are the processes independent? Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 785-801.

Bower, J. D., Bian, Z., & Andersen, G. J. (2012). Effects of retinal eccentricity and acuity on global-motion processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 942-949.

Bowling, A. C., Hindman, E. A., & Donnelly, J. F. (2012). Prosaccade errors in the antisaccade task: differences between corrected and uncorrected errors and links to neuropsychological tests. Experimental Brain Research, 216, 169-179.

Braun, D. I., Schütz, A. C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Localization of speed differences of context stimuli during fixation and smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Research, 50, 2740-2749.

Breen, M., & Clifton, C. Jr. (2011). Stress matters: Effects of anticipated lexical stress on silent reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 153-170.

Breen, M., & Clifton, C. Jr. (2013). Stress matters revisited: A boundary change experiment. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1896-1909.

Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Datta, R., Lewis, J. W., & Deyoe, E. A.(2009). The topography of visuospatial attention as revealed by a novel visual field mapping technique. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1447-1460.

Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106, 1465-1477.

Brenner, E., & Cornelissen, F. W. (2000). Separating simultaneous processing of egocentric and relative positions. Vision Research, 40, 2557-2563.

Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2007). Flexibility in intercepting moving objects. Journal of Vision, 7(5):14, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/7/5/14/, doi:10.1167/7.5.14.

Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2009). Sources of variability in interceptive movements. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 117-133.

Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2011). Continuous visual control of interception. Human Movement Science, 30, 475-494.

Brenner, E., Meijer, W. J., & Cornelissen, F. W. (2005). Judging relative positions across saccades. Vision Research, 45, 1587-1602.

Breslow, L. A., Trafton, G. J., & Ratwani, R. M. (2009). A perceptual process approach to selecting color scales for complex visualizations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 25-34.

Bricolo, E., Gianesini, T., Fanini, A., Bundesen, C., & Chelazzi, L. (2002). Serial attention mechanisms in visual search: A direct behavioral demonstration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 980-993.

Bridge, H., Hicks, S. L., Xie, J., Okell, T. W., Mannan, S., Alexander, I., Cowey, A., & Kennard, C. (2010). Visual activation of extra-striate cortex in the absence of V1 activation. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4148-4154.

Brisson, J., Mainville, M., Mailloux, D., Beaulieu, C., Serres, J., & Sirois, S. (2013). Pupil diameter measurement errors as a function of gaze direction in corneal reflection eyetrackers. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1322-1331.

Britt, A. E., Mirman, D., Kornilov, S, A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2014). Effect of repetition proportion on language-driven anticipatory eye movements. Acta Psychologica, 145, 128-138.

Brock, J. & Bzishvili, S. (2013). Deconstructing Frith and Snowling's homograph-reading task: Implications for autism spectrum disorders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1764-1773.

Brockmole, J. R., & Vo, M. L.-H. (2010). Semantic memory for contextual regularities within and across scene categories: Evidence from eye movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1803-1813.

Brockmore, J. R., & Boot, W. R. (2009). Should I stay or should I go? Attentional disengagement from visually unique and unexpected items at fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 808-815.

Brockmore, J. R., & Irwin, D. E. (2005). Eye movements and the integration of visual memory and visual perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 495-512.

Brooks, J. L., Gilaie-Dotan, S., Rees, G., Bentin, S., & Driver, J. (2012). Preserved local but disrupted contextual figure-ground influences in an individual with abnormal function of intermediate visual areas. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1393-1407.

Brouwer, A.-M., Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2009). Differences in fixations between grasping and viewing objects. Journal of Vision, 9(1):18, 1-24, http://journalofvision.org/9/1/18/, doi:10.1167/9.1.18.

Brouwer, G. J., & van Ee, R. (2006). Endogenous influences on perceptual bistability depends on exogenous stimulus characteristics. Vision Research, 46, 3393-3402.

Brouwer, G. J., & van Ee, R. (2007). Visual cortex allows prediction of perceptual states during ambiguous structure-from-motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1015-1023.

Brouwer, S., Mitterer, H., & Huettig, F. (2012). Can hearing puter activate pupil? Phonological competition and the processing of reduced spoken words in spontaneous conversations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 2193-2220.

Brouwer, S., Mitterer, H., & Huettig, F. (2012). Speech reductions change the dynamics of competition during spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 57, 539-571.

Brouwer, S., Mitterer, H., & Huettig, F. (2013). Discourse context and the recognition of reduced and canonical spoken words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 519-539.

Brown, H. R., & Friston, K. J. (2013). The functional anatomy of attention: a DCM study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:784. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00784.

Brown, M., Salverda, A. P., Dilley, L. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). Expectations from preceding prosody influence segmentation in online sentence processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1189-1196.

Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 171-190.

Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 893-900.

Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012). Beyond common and privileged: Gradient representations of common ground in real-time language use. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 62-89.

Brown-Schmidt, S., & Konopka, A. E. (2011). Experimental approaches to referential domains and the on-line processing of referring expressions in unscripted conversation. Information, 2, 302-326; doi:10.3390/info2020302.

Brown-Schmidt, S., Gunlogson, C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation. Cognition, 107, 1122-1134.

Brozzoli, C., Pavani, F., Urquizar, C., Cardinali, L., & Farne, A. (2009). Grasping actions remap peripersonal space. NeuroReport, 20, 913-917.

Bruchmann, M., Hintze, P., & Mota, S. (2011). The effects of spatial and temporal cueing on metacontrast masking. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 132-141.

Bruhn, P., & Bundesen, C. (2012). Anticipation of visual form independent of knowing where the form will occur. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 930-941.

Brunet, P. M., Heisz, J. J., Mondloch, C. J., Shore, D. I., & Schmidt, L. A. (2009). Shyness and face scanning in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 909-914.

Brzezicka, A., Krejtz, I., von Hecker, U., & Laubrock, J. (2012). Eye movement evidence for defocused attention in dysphoria: A perceptual span analysis. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85, 129-133.

Buchan, J. N., & Munhall, K. G. (2011). The influence of selective attention to auditory and visual speech on the integration of audiovisual speech information. Perception, 40, 1164-1182.

Buchan, J. N., & Munhall, K. G. (2012). The effect of a concurrent working memory task and temporal offsets on the integration of auditory and visual speech information. Seeing and Perceiving, 25, 87-106.

Buchan, J. N., Paré, M., & Munhall, K. G. (2007). Spatial statistics of gaze fixations during dynamic face processing. Social Neuroscience, 2, 1-13.

Buchan, J. N., Paré, M., & Munhall, K. G. (2008). The effect of varying talker identity and listening conditions on gaze behavior during audiovisual speech perception. Brain Research, 1242, 162-171.

Buetti, S., & Kerzel, D. (2010). Effects of saccades and response type on the Simon effect: If you look at the stimulus, the Simon effect may be gone. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2172-2189.

Buetti, S., Juan, E., Rinck, M., & Kerzel, D. (2012). Affective states leak into movement execution: Automatic avoidance of threatening stimuli in fear of spider is visible in reach trajectories. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 1176-1188.

Bull, S., Cooke, N., & Mabbott, A. (2007). Visual attention in open learner model presentations: An eye-tracking investigation. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4511, 177-186.

Bultitude, J. H., Van der Stigchel, S., & Nijboer, T. C. W. (2013). Prism adaptation alters spatial remapping in healthy individuals: Evidence from double-step saccades. Cortex, 49, 759-770.

Buonocore, A., & McIntosh, R. D. (2008). Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 117-122.

Buonocore, A., & McIntosh, R. D. (2012). Modulation of saccadic inhibition by distractor size and location. Vision Research, 69, 32-41.

Buonocore, A., & McIntosh, R. D. (2013). Attention modulates saccadic inhibition magnitude. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1051-1059.

Burge, W.K., Ross, L.A., Amthor, F.R., Mitchell, W.G., Zotov, A., & Visscher, K.M. (2013). Processing speed training increases the efficiency of attentional resource allocation in young adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:684. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00684.

Burke, M. R., Allen, R. J., & Gonzalez, C. (2012). Eye and hand movements during reconstruction of spatial memory. Perception, 41, 803-818.

Burke, M. R., Bramley, P., Gonzalez, C. C., & McKeefry, D. J. (2013). The contribution of the right supra-marginal gyrus to sequence learning in eye movements. Neuropsychologia, 51, 3048-3056.

Burton, C., & Daneman, M. (2007). Compensating for a limited working memory capacity during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Reading Psychology, 28, 163-186.

Burton, R., Crabb, D. P., Smith, N. D., Glen, F. C., & Garway-Heath, D. F. (2012). Glaucoma and reading: exploring the effects of contrast lowering of text. Optometry and Vision Science, 89,1282-1287.

Buschman, T. J., Denovellis, E. L., Diogo, C., Bullock, D., & Miller, E. K. (2012). Synchronous oscillatory neural ensembles for rules in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 76, 838-846.

Bushnell, B. N., & Pasupathy, A. (2012). Shape encoding consistency across colors in primate V4. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 1299-1308.

Bushnell, B. N., Harding, P. J., Kosai, Y., & Pasupathy, A. (2011). Partial occlusion modulates contour-based shape encoding in primate area V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4012-4024.

Bushnell, B. N., Harding, P. J., Kosai, Y., Bair, W., & Pasupathy, A. (2011). Equiluminance cells in visual cortical area V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 12398-12412.

Butler, S. H., Gilchrist, I. D., Ludwig, C. J. H., Muir, K., & Harvey, M. (2006). Impairments of oculomotor control in a patient with a right temporo-parietal lesion. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 990-999.

Butler, S. H., Rossit, S., Gilchrist, I. D., Ludwig, C. J. H., Olk, B., Muir, K., Reeves, I., & Harvey, M. (2009). Non-lateralised deficits in anti-saccade performance in patients with hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2488-2495.

Butler, S., Gilchrist, I. D., Burt, D. M., Perrett, D. I., Jones, E., & Harvey, M. (2005). Are the perceptual biases found in chimeric face processing reflected in eye-movement patterns? Neuropsychologia, 43, 52-59.

Byrne, P. A., Cappadocia, D. C., & Crawford, D. (2010). Interactions between gaze-centered and allocentric representations of reach target location in the presence of spatial updating. Vision Research, 50, 2661-2670.

Bélanger, N. N., & Rayner, K. (2013). Frequency and predictability effects in eye fixations for skilled and less-skilled deaf readers. Visual Cognition, 21, 477-497.

Bélanger, N. N., Mayberry, R. I., & Rayner, K. (2013). Orthographic and phonological preview benefits: Parafoveal processing in skilled and less-skilled deaf readers. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2237-2252.

Bélanger, N. N., Slattery, T. J., Mayberry, R. I., & Rayner K. (2012). Skilled deaf readers have an enhanced perceptual span in reading. Psychological Science, 23, 816-823.

Böhme, M., Dorr, M., Krause, C., Martinetz, T., & Barth, E. (2006). Eye movement predictions on natural videos. Neurocomputing, 69, 1996-2004.

Bölte, J., Böhl, A., Dobel, C., & Zwitserlood, P. (2009). Effects of referential ambiguity, time constraints and addressee orientation on the production of morphologically complex words. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1166-1199.

Back To Top

C

Cabel, D. W. J. , Armstrong, I. T. , Reingold, E. , & Munoz, D. P. (2000). Control of saccade initiation in a countermanding task using visual and auditory stop signals. Experimental Brain Research, 133, 431-441.

Caddigan, E., & Lleras, A. (2010). Saccadic repulsion in pop-out search: How a target’s dodgy history can push the eyes away from it. Journal of Vision, 10(14):9, 1-9, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/9, doi:10.1167/10.14.9.

Cai, X., & Padoa-Schioppa, C. (2012). Neuronal encoding of subjective value in dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 3791 - 3808.

Caldara, R., & Miellet, S. (2011). iMap: a novel method for statistical fixation mapping of eye movement data. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 864-878.

Caldara, R., Zhou, X., & Miellet, S. (2010). Putting culture under the 'spotlight' reveals universal information use for face recognition. PLoS ONE 5(3): e9708. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009708.

Callan, M. J., Ferguson, H. J., & Bindemann, M. (2013). Eye movements to audiovisual scenes reveal expectations of a just world. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 34-40.

Callejas, A., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2014). Dorsal and ventral attention systems underlie social and symbolic cueing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 63-80.

Calow, D., & Lappe, M. (2007). Local statistics of retinal optic flow for self-motion through natural sceneries. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 18, 343-374.

Calvo, M. G., & Avero, P. (2008). Affective priming of emotional pictures in parafoveal vision: Left visual field advantage. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8:41-53.

Calvo, M. G., & Castillo, M. D. (2009). Semantic word priming in the absence of eye fixations: Relative contributions of overt and covert attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 51-56.

Calvo, M. G., & Eysenck, M. W. (2008). Affective significance enhances covert attention: Roles of anxiety and word familiarity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1669-1686.

Calvo, M. G., & Fernández-Martín, A. (2013). Can the eyes reveal a person’s emotions? Biasing role of the mouth expression. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 202-211.

Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2007). Processing of unattended emotional visual scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 347-369.

Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2008). Detection of emotional faces: Salient physical features guide effective visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 471-494.

Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2009). Eye-movement assessment of the time course in facial expression recognition: Neurophysiological implications. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 398-411.

Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2009). Lateralised covert attention in word identification. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 14, 178-195.

Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2011). Time course of discrimination between emotional facial Expressions: The role of visual saliency. Vision Research, 51, 1751-1759.

Calvo, M. G., Fernández-Martín, A., & Nummenmaa, L. (2013). A smile biases the recognition of eye expressions: Configural projection from a salient mouth. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1159-1181.

Calvo, M. G., Gutierrez-Garcia, A., Avero, P., & Lundqvist, D. (2013). Attentional mechanisms in judging genuine and fake smiles: Eye-movement patterns. Emotion, 13, 792-802.

Calvo, M. G., Gutiérrez, A., & Fernández-Martín, A. (2012). Anxiety and deficient inhibition of threat distractors: Spatial attention span and time course. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 66-78.

Calvo, M. G., Nummenmaa, L., & Avero, P. (2008). Visual search of emotional faces: Eye-movement assessment of component processes. Experimental Psychology, 55, 359-370.

Calvo, M. G., Nummenmaa, L., & Avero, P. (2010). Recognition advantage of happy faces in extrafoveal vision: Featural and affective processing. Visual Cognition, 18, 1274-1297.

Calvo, M. G., Nummenmaa, L., & Hyönä, J. (2007). Emotional and neutral scenes in competition: Orienting, efficiency, and identification. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1585-1593.

Calvo, M. G., Nummenmaa, L., & Hyönä, J. (2008). Emotional scenes in peripheral vision: Selective orienting and gist processing, but not content identification. Emotion, 8, 68-80.

Camalier, C. R., Gotler, A., Murthy, A., Thompson, K. G., Logan, G. D., Palmeri, T. J., & Schall, J. D. (2007). Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque. Vision Research, 47, 2187-2211.

Camara, E., Manohar, S., & Husain, M. (2013). Past rewards capture spatial attention and action choices. Experimental Brain Research, 230, 291-300.

Camblin, C. C., Gordon, P.C., & Swaab, T. Y. (2007). The interplay of discourse congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: Evidence from ERPs and eye tracking. Journal of Memory & Language, 56, 103-128.

Cameron, I.G.M., Brien, D.C., Links, K., Robichaud, S., Ryan, J.D., Munoz, D.P., & Chow, T.W. (2013). Changes to saccade behaviors in Parkinson’s disease following dancing and observation of dancing. Frontiers in Neurology, 4:22. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00022.

Campbell, K. L., & Ryan, J. D. (2009). The effects of practice and external support on older adults' control of reflexive eye movements. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 16, 745 - 763.

Campbell, K. L., Al-Aidroos, N., Fatt, R., Pratt, J., & Hasher, L. (2010). The effects of multisensory targets on saccadic trajectory deviations: eliminating age differences. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 385-392.

Campbell, K. L., Al-Aidroos, N., Pratt, J., & Hasher, L. (2009). Repelling the young and attracting the old: Examining age-related differences in saccade trajectory deviations. Psychology and Aging, 24, 163-168.

Campbell, L., McCabe, K., Leadbeater, K., Schall, U., Loughland, C., & Rich, D. (2010). Visual scanning of faces in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: Attention to the mouth or the eyes? Psychiatry Research, 177, 211-215.

Cane, J. E., Cauchard, F., & Weger, U. W. (2012). The time-course of recovery from interruption during reading: Eye movement evidence for the role of interruption lag and spatial memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1397-1413.

Canestrelli, A. R., Mak, W. M., & Sanders, T. J. M. (2013). Causal connectives in discourse processing: How differences in subjectivity are reflected in eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1394-1413.

Canham, M., & Hegarty, M. (2010). Effects of knowledge and display design on comprehension of complex graphics. Learning and Instruction, 20, 155-166.

Capilla, A., Belin, P., & Gross, J. (2013). The early spatio-temporal correlates and task independence of cerebral voice processing studied with MEG. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1388-1395.

Caplan, D. (2010). Task effects on BOLD signal correlates of implicit syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 866-890.

Caplovitz, G. P., & Tse, P. U. (2007). Rotating dotted ellipses: Motion perception driven by grouped figural rather than local dot motion signals. Vision Research, 47, 979-1991.

Caplovitz, G. P., & Tse, P. U. (2007). V3A processes contour curvature as a trackable feature for the perception of rotational motion. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1179-1189.

Caplovitz, G. P., Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2006). Mechanisms underlying the perceived angular velocity of a rigidly rotating object. Vision Research, 46, 2877-2893.

Caplovitz, G. P., Paymer, N. A., & Tse, P. U. (2008). The Drifting Edge Illusion: A stationary edge abutting an oriented drifting grating appears to move because of the 'other aperture problem. Vision Research, 48, 2403-2414.

Carbone, E., & Schneider, W. X. (2010). The control of stimulus-driven saccades is subject not to central, but to visual attention limitations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2168-2175.

Cardoso de Oliveira, S., & Barthélémy, S. (2005). Visual feedback reduces bimanual coupling of movement amplitudes, but not of directions. Experimental Brain Research, 162, 78-88.

Cardoso-Leite, P., & Gorea, A. (2009). Comparison of perceptual and motor decisions via confidence judgements and saccade curvature. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 2822-2836.

Carletta, J., Hill, R. L., Nicol, C., Taylor, T., de Ruiter, J. P., & Bard, E. G. (2010). Eyetracking for two-person tasks with manipulation of a virtual world. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 254-265.

Carminati, M. N., van Gompel, R. P. G., Scheepers, C., & Arai, M. (2008). Syntactic priming in comprehension: The role of argument order and animacy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 1098-1110.

Carminati, M.N., & Knoeferle, P. (2013). Effects of speaker emotional facial expression and listener age on incremental sentence processing. PLoS ONE 8(9): e72559. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072559.

Carriere, J. S. A., Eaton, D., Reynolds, M. G., Dixon, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2009). Grapheme-color synesthesia influences overt visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 246-258.

Caspi, A., Beutter, B. R., & Eckstein, M. P. (2004). The time course of visual information accrual guiding eye movement decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 13086-13090.

Cassavaugh, N. D., Kramer, A. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2003). Influence of task-irrelevant onset distractors on the visual search performance of young and old adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10, 44-60.

Cassavaugh, N., Kramer, A. F., Peterson, M. S. (2004). Aging and the strategic control of the fixation offset effect. Psychology and Aging, 19, 357-361.

Cassey, T.C., Evens, D.R., Bogacz, R., Marshall, J.A.R., & Ludwig, C.J.H. (2013). Adaptive sampling of information in perceptual decision-making. PLoS ONE 8(11): e78993. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078993.

Castelhano, M. S. & Heaven, C. (2011). Scene context influences without scene gist: Eye movements guided by spatial associations in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 890-896.

Castelhano, M. S., & Heaven, C. (2010). The relative contribution of scene context and target features to visual search in real-world scenes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1283-1297.

Castelhano, M. S., Pollatsek, A., & Cave, K. R. (2008). Typicality aids search for an unspecified target, but only in identification and not in attentional guidance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 795-801.

Cauchard, F., Cane, J. E., & Weger, U. W. (2012). Influence of background speech and music in interrupted reading: An eye-tracking study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 381-390.

Cavina-Pratesi, C. & Hesse, C. (2013). Why do the eyes prefer the index finger? Simultaneous recording of eye and hand movements during precision grasping. Journal of Vision, 13(5):15, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/5/15, doi:10.1167/13.5.15.

Cavézian, C., Valadao, D., Hurwitz, M., Saoud, M., & Danckert, J. (2012). Finding centre: Ocular and fMRI investigations of bisection and landmark task performance. Brain Research, 1437, 89-103.

Cazzoli, D., Nyffeler, T., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2011). Vertical bias in neglect: A question of time? Neuropsychologia, 49, 2369-74.

Cañal-Bruland, R., Lotz, S., Hagemann, N., Schorer, J., & Strauss, B. (2011). Visual span and change detection in soccer: An expertise study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 302-310.

Cerf, M., Frady, E. P., & Koch, C. (2009). Faces and text attract gaze independent of the task: Experimental data and computer model. Journal of Vision, 9(12):10, 1-15, http://journalofvision.org/9/12/10/, doi:10.1167/9.12.10.

Cerf, M., Harel, J., Huth, A., Einhäuser, W., & Koch, C. (2009). Decoding what people see from where they look: Predicting visual stimuli from scanpaths. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5395, 15-26.

Cesqui, B., van de Langenberg, R., Lacquaniti, F., & d’Avella, A. (2013). A novel method for measuring gaze orientation in space in unrestrained head conditions. Journal of Vision, 13(8):28, 1-22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/28,doi:10.1167/13.8.28.

Chahine, G., & Krekelberg, B. (2009). Cortical contributions to saccadic suppression. PLoS ONE 4(9): e6900. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006900.

Chamberland, C., Saint-Aubin, J., & Legere, M.-A. (2013). The impact of text repetition on content and function words during reading: Further evidence from eye movements. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 94-99.

Chambers, C. G., & Cooke, H. (2009). Lexical competition during second-language listening: Sentence context, but not proficiency, constrains interference from the native lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1029-1040.

Champion, R. A., & Freeman, T. C. A. (2010). Discrimination contours for the perception of head-centered velocity. Journal of Vision, 10(6):14, 1-9, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/14, doi:10.1167/10.6.14.

Chan, F., Armstrong, I. T., Pari, G., Riopelle, R. J., & Munoz, D. P. (2005). Deficits in saccadic eye-movement control in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 43, 784-796.

Chan, J. P. K., & Ryan, J. D. (2012). Holistic representations of internal and external face features are used to support recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:87. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00087.

Chan, J. P. K., Kamino, D., Binns, M. A., & Ryan, J. D. (2011) Can changes in eye movement scanning alter the age-related deficit in recognition memory?. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:92. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00092.

Chanceaux, M., & Grainger, J. (2012). Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, symbols, and shapes in peripheral vision. Acta Psychologica, 141, 149-158.

Chanceaux, M., & Grainger, J. (2013). Constraints on letter-in-string identification in peripheral vision: effects of number of flankers and deployment of attention. Frontiers in Psycholology, 4:119. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00119.

Chanceaux, M., Guérin-Dugué, A., Lemaire, B., & Baccino, T. (2008). Towards a model of information seeking by integrating visual, semantic and memory maps. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5329, 65-78.

Chanceaux, M., Guérin-Dugué, A., Lemaire, B., & Baccino, T. (2009). A model to simulate web users' eye movements. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5726, 288-300.

Chanceaux, M., Mathôt, S., & Grainger, J. (2013). Flank to the left, flank to the right: Testing the modified receptive field hypothesis of letter-specific crowding. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 774-780.

Chanceaux, M., Vitu, F., Bendahman, L., Thorpe, S., & Grainger, J. (2012). Word processing speed in peripheral vision measured with a saccadic choice task. Vision Research, 56, 10-19.

Chang, C.C., Lee, Y.H., Lin, C.J., Liu, B.S., & Shih, Y.C. (2012). Visual assessment of camouflaged targets with different background similarities. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 114, 527-541.

Chang, S. W. C., Barter, J. W., Ebitz, R. B., Watson, K. K., & Platt, M. L. (2012). Inhaled oxytocin amplifies both vicarious reinforcement and self reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). PNAS, 109, 959-964.

Chang, S. W. C., Gariépy, J.-F., & Platt, M. L. (2013). Neuronal reference frames for social decisions in primate frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 16, 243-250.

Chang, S. W. C., Winecoff, A. A., & Platt, M. L. (2011). Vicarious reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5:27. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00027.

Chao, C.-M., Tseng, P., Hsu, T.-Y., Su, J.-H., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., Muggleton, N. G., & Juan, C.-H. (2011). Predictability of saccadic behaviors is modified by transcranial magnetic stimulation over human posterior parietal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 11, 1961-1972.

Chapman, C. S., Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2007). Squeezing uncertainty from saccadic compression. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(1):2, 1-5, http://jemr.org/.

Charness, N., Reingold, E. M., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). The perceptual aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1146-1152.

Chen, A., den Os, E., & de Ruiter, J. P. (2007). Pitch accent type matters for online processing of information status: Evidence from natural and synthetic speech. The Linguistic Review, 24, 317-344.

Chen, C.-Y., & Hafed, Z. M. (2013). Postmicrosaccadic enhancement of slow eye movements. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 5375-5386.

Chen, L., & Boland, J. (2008). Dominance and context effects on activation of alternative homophone meanings. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1306-1323.

Chen, L., Li, X., & Yang, Y. (2012). Focus, newness and their combination: Processing of information structure in discourse. PLoS ONE 7(8): e42533. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042533.

Chen, M., & Ko, H. (2011). Exploring the eye-movement patterns as Chinese children read texts: a developmental perspective. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 232-246.

Chen, Q., Weidner, R., Weiss, P. H., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2012). Neural interaction between spatial domain and spatial reference frame in parietal-occipital junction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 2223-2236.

Chen, W.-Y., Howe, P. D., Holcombe, A. O. (2013). Resource demands of object tracking and differential allocation of the resource. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 710-725.

Chen, X., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Real-world visual search is dominated by top-down guidance. Vision Research, 46, 4118-4133.

Chen, X., Han, F., Poo, M.-M., & Dan, Y. (2007). Excitatory and suppressive receptive field subunits in awake monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19120-19125.

Chen, X., Scangos, K. W., & Stuphorn, V. (2010). Supplementary motor area exerts proactive and reactive control of arm movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 14657-14675.

Chen, Y., Byrne, P., & Crawford, J. D. (2011). Time course of allocentric decay; egocentric decay; and allocentric-to-egocentric conversion in memory-guided reach. Neuropsychologia, 49, 49-60.

Chen, Y., Shi, R., & Tian, H. (2004). Optimal landing position in reading Chinese idioms. Psychological Science (China), 27, 278-280.

Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 104, 103-125.

Chesnet, D., & Alamargot, D. (2005). Analyse en temps réel des activités oculaires et grapho-motrices du scripteur. Intérêt du dispositif 'Eye and pen'. L'année psychologique, 105, 477-520.

Chesney, D. L., McNeil, N. M., Brockmole, J. R., & Kelley, K. (2013). An eye for relations: eye-tracking indicates long-term negative effects of operational thinking on understanding of math equivalence. Memory & Cognition, 41, 1079-1095.

Chettih, S., Durgin, F. H., & Grodner, D. J. (2012). Mixing metaphors in the cerebral hemispheres: What happens when careers collide? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 295-311.

Chi, E. H., Gumbrecht, M., & Hong, L. (2007). Visual foraging of highlighted text: An eye-tracking study. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4552, 589-598.

Chiau, H. Y., Tseng, P., Su, J.-H., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., Muggleton, N. G., & Juan, C. H. (2011). Trial type probability modulates the cost of antisaccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 515 - 526.

Chica, A. B., Klein, R. M., Rafal, R. D., & Hopfinger, J. B. (2010). Endogenous saccade preparation does not produce inhibition of return: Failure to replicate rafal, calabresi, brennan, & sciolto (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1193-206.

Chica, A. B., Taylor, T. L., Lupiáñez, J., & Klein, R. M. (2010). Two mechanisms underlying inhibition of return. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 25-35.

Chiew, K.S., & Braver, T.S. (2013). Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00015.

Chincotta, D., Hyönä, J., & Underwood, G. (1997). Eye fixations, speech rate and bilingual digit span: Numeral reading indexes fluency not word length. Acta Psychologica, 97, 253-275.

Chisholm, J. D., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Improved top-down control reduces oculomotor capture: The case of action video game players. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 257-262.

Choi, W., & Gordon, P. C. (2013). Coordination of word recognition and oculomotor control during reading: The role of implicit lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 1032-1046.

Christie, J., Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2013). Inhibition of return is at the midpoint of simultaneous cues. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1610-1618.

Chu, S., Paul, N., & Ruel, L. (2009). Using eye tracking technology to examine the effectiveness of design elements on news websites. Information Design Journal, 17, 31-43.

Chung, S. T. L., (2004). Reading speed benefits from increased vertical word spacing in normal peripheral vision. Optometry & Vision Science, 81, 525-535.

Chung, S. T. L., Legge, G. E., & Cheung, S.-H. (2004). Letter-recognition and reading speed in peripheral vision benefit from perceptual learning. Vision Research, 44, 695-709.

Churan, J,, Guitton, D., & Pack, C. C. (2012). Perisaccadic remapping and rescaling of visual responses in macaque superior colliculus. PLoS ONE 7(12): e52195. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052195.

Churan, J., Guitton, D., & Pack, C. C. (2011). Context dependence of receptive field remapping in superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 1862-1874.

Churan, J., Guitton, D., & Pack, C. C. (2012). Spatiotemporal structure of visual receptive fields in macaque superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 2653-2667.

Churan, J., Khawaja, F. A., Tsui, J. M. G, & Pack, C. C. (2008). Brief motion stimuli preferentially activate surround-suppressed neurons in macaque visual area MT. Current Biology, 18, R1051-1052.

Cirilli, L., de Timary, P., Lefèvre, P., & Missal, M. (2011). Individual differences in impulsivity predict anticipatory eye movements. PLoS ONE 6(10): e26699. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026699.

Clahsen, H., Balkhair, L., Schutter, J.-S., & Cunnings, I. (2013). The time course of morphological processing in a second language. Second Language Research, 29, 7-31.

Clarke, A.D.F., Coco, M.I., & Keller, F. (2013). The impact of attentional, linguistic, and visual features during object naming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:927. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00927.

Clayards, M., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Jacobs, R. A. (2008). Perception of speech reflects optimal use of probabilistic speech cues. Cognition, 108, 804-809.

Clemens, I. A. H., Selen, L. P. J., Koppen, M., & Medendorp, W. P. (2012). Visual stability across combined eye and body motion. Journal of Vision, 12(12):8, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/12/8, doi:10.1167/12.12.8.

Clifton, C. Jr. (2013). Situational context affects definiteness preferences: Accommodation of presuppositions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 487-501.

Clifton, C., Jr., & Frazier, L. (2012). Discourse integration guided by the "Question under Discussion". Cognitive Psychology, 65, 352-379.

Clifton, C., Jr., & Frazier, L. (2012). Interpreting conjoined noun phrases and conjoined clauses: Collective vs. distributive preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1760-1776.

Coco, M. I., & Keller, F. (2012). Scan patterns predict sentence production in the cross-modal processing of visual scenes. Cognitive Science, 36, 1204-1223.

Coeckelbergh, T. R. M., Cornelissen, F. W., Brouwer, W. H., & Kooijman, A. C. (2002). The effect of visual field defects on eye movements and practical fitness to drive. Vision Research, 42, 669-677.

Coesmans, M., Röder, C.H., Smit, A.E., Koekkoek, S.K., De Zeeuw, C.I., Frens, M.A., & van der Geest, J.N.. (2014). Cerebellar motor learning deficits in medicated and medication-free men with recent-onset schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience, 39, E3-E11.

Coesmans, M., Smitt, P. A. S., Linden, D. J., Shigemoto, R., Hirano, T., Yamakawa, Y., Van Alphen, A. M., Luo, C., Van Der Geest, J. N., Kros, J. M., Gaillard, C. A., Frens, M. A., & De Zeeuw, C. I. (2003). Mechanisms underlying cerebellar motor deficits due to mGluR1-autoantibodies. Annals of Neurology, 53, 325-336.

Coffman, B. A., Kodituwakku, P., Kodituwakku, E. L., Romero, L., Sharadamma, N. M., Stone, D., & Stephen, J. M. (2013). Primary visual response (M100) delays in adolescents with FASD as measured with MEG. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 2852-2862.

Cohen, A. L. (2013). Software for the automatic correction of recorded eye fixation locations in reading experiments. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 679-683.

Colas, F., Flacher, F., Tanner, T., Bessière, P., & Girard, B. (2009). Bayesian models of eye movement selection with retinotopic maps. Biological Cybernetics, 100, 203-214.

Colcombe, A. M., Kramer, A. F., Irwin, D. E., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, S., & Hahn, S. (2003). Age-related effects of attentional and oculomotor capture by onsets and color singletons as a function of experience. Acta Psychologica, 113, 205-226.

Cole, G. G., & Kuhn, G. (2009). Appearance matters: Attentional orienting by new objects in the precueing paradigm. Visual Cognition, 17, 755-776.

Collin, C. A., & Chauduri, A. (2002). Using MATLAB with the Psychophysics Toolbox to present the heterochromatic fusion nystagmus stimulus. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 500-508.

Collin, C. A., McMullen, P. A., Séguin, J.-A. (2009). A significant bilateral field advantage for shapes defined by static and motion cues. Perception, 38, 1132-1143.

Collins, T. (2010). Extraretinal signal metrics in multiple-saccade sequences. Journal of Vision, 10(14):7, 1-14,http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/7, doi:10.1167/10.14.7.

Collins, T. (2012). Probability of seeing increases saccadic readiness. PLoS One 7(11): e49454. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049454.

Collins, T., & Wallman, J. (2012). The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 3342-3348.

Collins, T., Doré-Mazars, K., & Lappe, M. (2007). Motor space structures perceptual space: Evidence from human saccadic adaptation. Brain Research, 1172, 32-39.

Collins, T., Heed, T., & Röder, B. (2010). Eye-movement driven changes in the perception of auditory space. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 736-746.

Collins, T., Heed, T., & Röder, B. (2010). Visual target selection and motor planning define attentional enhancement at perceptual processing stages. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 4:14. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.014.2010

Collins, T., Heed, T., Doré-Mazars, K., & Röder, B. (2010). Presaccadic attention interferes with feature detection. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 111-117.

Collins, T., Rolfs, M., Deubel, H., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). Post-saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non-foveal locations. Journal of Vision, 9(5):29, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/9/5/29/, doi:10.1167/9.5.29.

Collins, T., Schicke, T., & Röder, B. (2008). Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition, 109, 363-371.

Colonius, H., & Diederich, A. (2004). Multisensory interaction in saccadic reaction time: A time-window-of-integration model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1000-1009.

Conati, C., Merten, C., Muldner, K., & Ternes, D. (2005). Exploring eye tracking to increase bandwidth in user modeling. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 3538, 357-366.

Contreras, R., Ghajar, J., Bahar, S., & Suh, M. (2011). Effect of cognitive load on eye-target synchronization during smooth pursuit eye movement. Brain Research, 1398, 55-63.

Contreras, R., Kolster, R., Voss, H. U., Ghajar, J., Suh, M., & Bahar, S. (2008). Eye-target synchronization in mild traumatic brain-injured patients. Journal of Biological Physics, 34, 381-392.

Coppe, S., Orban de Xivry, J.-J., Missal, M., & Lefèvre, P. (2010). Biological motion influences the visuomotor transformation for smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Research, 50, 2721-2728.

Coppe, S., Orban de Xivry, J.-J., Yüksel, D., Ivanoiu, A., & Lefèvre, P. (2012). Dramatic impairment of prediction due to frontal lobe degeneration. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 2957-2966.

Corbett, J. E., & Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual performance fields: Frames of reference. PLoS ONE 6(9): e24470. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024470.

Corley, M. (2010). Making predictions from speech with repairs: evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 706-727.

Cornelissen, F. W., & van den Dobbelsteen, J. J. (1999). Heading detection with simulated visual field defects. Visual Impairment Research, 1, 71-84.

Cornelissen, F. W., Bruin, K. J., & Kooijman, A. C. (2005). The influence of artificial scotomas on eye-movements during visual search. Optometry & Vision Science, 82, 27-35.

Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E. M., & Palmer, J. (2002). The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behavioral Research Methods, Instrumentation and Computers, 34, 613-617.

Costello, M. G., Zhu, D., Salinas, E., & Stanford, T. R. (2013). Perceptual modulation of motor - but not visual - responses in the frontal eye field during an urgent-decision task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 16394-16408.

Cotti, J., Guillaume, A., Alahyane, N., Pélisson, D., & Vercher, J. -L. (2007). Adaptation of voluntary saccades, but not of reactive saccades, transfers to hand pointing movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 602-612.

Cotti, J., Panouilleres, M., Munoz, D. P., Vercher, J. -L., Pélisson, D., & Guillaume, A. (2009). Adaptation of reactive and voluntary saccades: different patterns of adaptation revealed in the antisaccade task. Journal of Physiology, 587, 127-138.

Cotti, J., Rohenkohl, G., Stokes, M., Nobre, A. C., & Coull, J. T. (2011). Functionally dissociating temporal and motor components of response preparation in left intraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage, 54, 1221-1230.

Cotti, J., Vercher, J.-L., & Guillaume, A. (2011). Hand-eye coordination relies on extra-retinal signals: evidence from reactive saccade adaptation. Behavioural Brain Research, 218, 248-252.

Coulson, S., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2006). Looking back: Joke comprehension and the space structuring model. Humor - International Journal of Humor Research, 19, 229-250.

Coutrot, A., Guyader, N., Ionescu, G., & Caplier, A. (2012). Influence of soundtrack on eye movements during video exploration. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(4):2, 1-10.

Coventry, K. R., Lynott, D., Cangelosi, A., Monrouxe, L., Joyce, D., & Richardson, D. C. (2010). Spatial language, visual attention, and perceptual simulation. Brain and Language, 112, 202-213.

Cowper-Smith, C. D., & Westwood, D. A. (2013). Motor IOR revealed for reaching. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1914-1922.

Cowper-Smith, C. D., Eskes, G. A., & Westwood, D. A. (2013). Motor inhibition of return can affect prepared reaching movements. Neuroscience Letters, 541, 83-86.

Cowper-Smith, C. D., Eskes, G. A., Westwood, D. A. (2012). Saccadic inhibition of return can arise from late-stage execution processes. Neuroscience Letters, 531, 120-124.

Cowper-Smith, C. D., Lau, E. Y. Y., Helmick, C. A., Eskes, G. A., & Westwood, D. A. (2010). Neural coding of movement direction in the healthy human brain. PLoS ONE 5(10): e13330. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013330.

Cowper-Smith, C.D., Harris, J., Eskes, G.A., & Westwood, D.A. (2013). Spatial interactions between successive eye and arm movements: Signal Type Matters. PLoS ONE 8(3): e58850. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058850.

Cox, D. D., Papanastassiou, A. M., Oreper, D., Andken, B. B., & DiCarlo, J. (2008). High-resolution three-dimensional microelectrode brain mapping using stereo microfocal X-Ray imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 2966-2976.

Coy, A. L., & Hutton, S. B. (2012). The influence of extrafoveal emotional faces on prosaccade latencies. Visual Cognition, 20, 883-901.

Coy, A. L., & Hutton, S. B. (2013). Lateral asymmetry in saccadic eye movements during face processing: The role of individual differences in schizotypy. Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 66-72.

Cozijn, R., Commandeur, E., Vonk, W., & Noordman, L. G. M. (2011). The time course of the use of implicit causality information in the processing of pronouns: A visual world paradigm study. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 381-403.

Crabb, D. P., Smith, N. D., Rauscher, F. G., Chisholm, C. M., Barbur, J. L., Edgar, D. F., & Garway-Heath, D. F. (2010). Exploring eye movements in patients with glaucoma when viewing a driving scene. PLoS ONE 5(3): e9710. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009710.

Crawford, M. T., Skowronski, J. J., Stiff, C., & Leonards, U. (2008). Seeing, but not thinking: Limiting the spread of spontaneous trait transference II. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 840-847.

Crawford, T. J., Parker, E., Solis-Trapala, I., & Mayes, J. (2011). Is the relationship of prosaccade reaction times and antisaccade errors mediated by working memory? Experimental Brain Research, 208, 385-397.

Creel, S. C. (2012). Phonological similarity and mutual exclusivity: on-line recognition of atypical pronunciations in 3-5-year-olds. Developemental Science, 15, 697-713.

Creel, S. C. (2012). Preschoolers' use of talker information in on-line comprehension. Child Development, 83, 2042-2056.

Creel, S. C. (2014). Impossible to _gnore: Word-form inconsistency slows preschool children's word-learning. Language Learning and Development, 10, 68-95.

Creel, S. C., & Tumlin, M. A. (2011). On-line acoustic and semantic interpretation of talker information. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 264-285.

Creel, S. C., & Tumlin, M. A. (2012). Online recognition of music is influenced by relative and absolute pitch information. Cognitive Science, 36, 224-260.

Creel, S. C., Aslin, R. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Heeding the voice of experience: The role of talker variation in lexical access. Cognition, 106, 633-664.

Creel, S. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2006). Consequences of lexical stress on learning an artificial lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 15-32.

Cristino, F., & Baddeley, R. (2009). The nature of the visual representations involved in eye movements when walking down the street. Visual Cognition, 17, 880-903.

Cristino, F., Mathôt, S., Theeuwes, J., & Gilchrist, I.D. (2010). ScanMatch: a novel method for comparing fixation sequences. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 692-700.

Cronin, T. H., Hertle, R. W., Ishikawa, H., & Schuman, J. S. (2009). Spectral domain optical coherence tomography for detection of foveal morphology in patients with nystagmus. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 13, 563-566.

Crossland, M. D., & Rubin, G. S. (2002). The use of an infra-red eyetracker to measure fixation stability. Optometry and Vision Science, 79, 735-739.

Crossland, M. D., & Rubin, G. S. (2006). Eye movements and reading in macular disease: Further support for the shrinking perceptual span hypothesis. Vision Research, 46, 590-597.

Crossland, M. D., Culham, L. E., & Rubin, G. S. (2004). Fixation stability and reading speed in patients with newly developed macular disease. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 24, 327-333.

Crossland, M. D., Culham, L. E., & Rubin, G. S. (2005). Reading speed and the perceptual span in patients with macular disease. International Congress Series, 1282, 498-501.

Crossland, M. D., Morland, A. B., Feely, M. P., von dem Hagen, E., & Rubin, G. S. (2008). The effect of age and fixation instability on retinotopic mapping of Primary Visual Cortex. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 49, 3734-3739.

Crossland, M. D., Sims, M., Galbraith, R. F., & Rubin, G. S. (2004). Evaluation of a new quantitative technique to assess the number and extent of preferred retinal loci in macular disease. Vision Research, 44, 1537-1546.

Crossland, M., Culham, L., Kabanarou, S., & Rubin, G. (2005). Preferred retinal locus development in patients with macular disease. Ophthalmology, 112, 1579-1585.

Cruickshank, A. G., & McSorley, E. (2009). Involuntary inhibition of movement initiation alters oculomotor competition resolution. Experimental Brain Research, 193, 467-476.

Crundall, D., Chapman, P., France, E., Underwood, G., & Phelps, N. (2005). What attracts attention during police pursuit driving? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 409-420.

Crundall, D., Chapman, P., Phelps, N., & Underwood, G. (2003). Eye movements and hazard perception in police pursuit and emergency response driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 163-174.

Crundall, D., Shenton, C., & Underwood, G. (2004). Eye movements during intentional car following. Perception, 33, 975-986.

Crundall, D., Van Loon, E., & Underwood, G. (2006). Attraction and distraction of attention with roadside advertisements. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38, 671-677.

Crutch, S. J., Lehmann, M., Gorgoraptis, N., Kaski, D., Ryan, N., Husain, M., & Warrington, E. K. (2011). Abnormal visual phenomena in posterior cortical atrophy. Neurocase: The Neural Basis of Cognition, 17, 160-177.

Cui, J., Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S. L., King, M., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2011). Social misdirection fails to enhance a magic illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5:103. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00103.

Cui, L., Drieghe, D., Yan, G., Bai, X., Chi, H., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Parafoveal processing across different lexical constituents in Chinese reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 403-416.

Cui, Y., Liu, D. L., Khawaja, F. A., Pack, C. C., & Butts, D. A. (2013). Diverse suppressive influences in area MT and selectivity to complex motion features. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 16715-16728.

Cunnings, I., & Clahsen, H. (2007). The time-course of morphological constraints: Evidence from eye-movements during reading. Cognition, 104, 476-494.

Cunnings, I., & Clahsen, H. (2008). The time-course of morphological constraints: A study of plurals inside derived words. The Mental Lexicon, 3, 149-175.

Cunnings, I., & Felser, C. (2013). The role of working memory in the processing of reflexives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 188-219.

Cunnings, I., Patterson, C., & Felser, C. (2014).Variable binding and coreference in sentence comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 712, 39-56.

Cushman, C. L., & Johnson, R. L. (2011). Age-of-acquisition effects in pure alexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1726-1742.

Cutler, A., Weber, A., & Otake, T. (2006). Asymmetric mapping from phonetic to lexical representations in second-language listening. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 269-284.

Cárdenas, R. A., Harris, L. J., & Becker, M. W. (2013). Sex differences in visual attention toward infant faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 280-287.

Cölln, M. C., Kusch, K., Helmert, J. R., Kohler, P., Velichkovsky, B. M., & Pannasch, S. (2012). Comparing two types of engineering visualizations: Task-related manipulations matter. Applied Ergonomics, 43, 48-56.

Back To Top

D

D'Ydewalle, G., & De Bruycker, W. (2007). Eye movements of children and adults while reading television subtitles. European Psychologist, 12, 196-205.

Dafoe, J., Armstrong, I., & Munoz, D. (2007). The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 179, 563-570.

Dagnino, B., Navajas, J., & Sigman, M. (2012). Eye fixations indicate men's preference for female breasts or buttocks. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 929-937.

Dahan, D., & Gaskell, M. G. (2007). The temporal dynamics of ambiguity resolution: Evidence from spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 483-501.

Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 498-513.

Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2005). Looking at the rope when looking for the snake: Conceptually mediated eye movements during spoken-word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 453-459.

Dahan, D., Drucker, S. J., & Scarborough, R. A. (2008). Talker adaptation in speech perception: Adjusting the signal or the representations? Cognition, 108, 710-718.

Dahlstrom-Hakki, I., & Pollatsek, A. (2006). Limits on integrating motion information across saccades. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 44-54.

Daini, R., Albonico, A., Malaspina, M., Martelli, M., Primativo, S., & Arduino, L. S. (2013). Dissociation in optokinetic stimulation sensitivity between omission and substitution reading errors in neglect dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:581. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00581.

Dalrymple, K. A., Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., Barton, J. J. S., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Experiencing simultanagnosia through windowed viewing of complex social scenes. Brain Research, 1367, 265-277.

Dalrymple, K. A., Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., Barton, J. J. S., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Opening a window on attention: Documenting and simulating recovery from simultanagnosia. Cortex, 47, 787-799.

Dalrymple, K. A., Bischof, W. F., Cameron, D., Barton, J. J. S., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Global perception in simultanagnosia is not as simple as a game of connect-the-dots. Vision Research, 49, 1901-1908.

Dalrymple, K. A., Bischof, W. F., Cameron, D., Barton, J. J. S., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Simulating simultanagnosia: Spatially constricted vision mimics local capture and the global processing deficit. Experimental Brain Research, 202, 445-455.

Dalrymple, K.A., Gray, A.K., Perler, B.L., Birmingham, E., Bischof, W.F., Barton, J.J., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Eyeing the eyes in social scenes: Evidence for top-down control of stimulus selection in simultanagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 25-40.

Dambacher, M., & Kliegl, R. (2007). Synchronizing timelines: Relations between fixation durations and N400 amplitudes during sentence reading. Brain Research, 1155, 147-162.

Dambacher, M., Slattery, T. J., Yang, J., Kliegl, R., & Rayner, K. (2013). Evidence for direct control of eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 1468-1484.

Dandekar, S., Ding, J., Privitera, C., Carney, T., & Klein, S. A. (2012). The fixation and saccade P3. PLoS ONE 7(11): e48761. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048761.

Dandekar, S., Privitera, C., Carney, T., & Klein, S. A. (2012). Neural saccadic response estimation during natural viewing. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 1776 - 1790.

Daneman, M., & Reingold, E. M. (2000). Do readers use phonological codes to activate word meanings? Evidence from eye movements. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 447-473). Elsevier: Amsterdam.

Daneman, M., Hannon, B., & Burton, C. (2006). Are there age-related differences in shallow semantic processing of text? Evidence from eye movements. Discourse Processes, 42, 177-203.

Daneman, M., Lennertz, T., & Hannon, B. (2007). Shallow semantic processing of text: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 83-105.

Dare, N., & Shillcock, R. (2013). Serial and parallel processing in reading: Investigating the effects of parafoveal orthographic information on nonisolated word recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 487-504.

Das, A., & Gambäck, B. (2013). Sentimental eyes! Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8008, 310-318.

Dassonville, P., & Bala, J. K. (2004). Perception, action, and Roelofs effect: A mere illusion of dissociation. PLoS Biology, 2, 1936-1945.

Dassonville, P., Bridgeman, B., Bala, J. K., Thiem, P., & Sampanes, A. (2004). The induced Roelofs effect: Two visual systems or the shift of a single reference frame? Vision Research, 44, 603-611.

Dauphin, B., & Greene, H. H. (2012). Here's looking at you: Eye movement exploration of rorschach images. Rorschachiana, 33, 3-22.

Davare, M., Zénon, A., Pourtois, G., Desmurget, M., & Olivier, E. (2012). Role of the medial part of the intraparietal sulcus in implementing movement direction. Cereb Cortex, 22, 1382 - 1394.

David, S. V., Hayden, B. Y., & Gallant, J. L. (2006). Spectral receptive field properties explain shape selectivity in Area V4. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 3492-3505.

David, S. V., Hayden, B. Y., Mazer, J. A., & Gallant, J. L. (2008). Attention to stimulus features shifts spectral tuning of V4 neurons during natural vision. Neuron, 59, 509-521.

Davidesco, I., Harel, M., Ramot, M., Kramer, U., Kipervasser, S., Andelman, F., Neufeld, M. Y., Goelman, G., Fried, I., & Malach, R. (2013). Spatial and object-based attention modulates broadband high-frequency responses across the human visual cortical hierarchy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1228-1240.

Davies, J., R., & Freeman, T. C. A. (2011). Simultaneous adaptation to non-collinear retinal motion and smooth pursuit eye movement. Vision Research, 51, 1637-1647.

Davis, C. J., Perea, M., & Acha, J. (2009). Re(de)fining the orthographic neighborhood: The role of addition and deletion neighbors in lexical decision and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 1550-1570.

Davis, S. L., Frohman, T. C., Crandall, C. G., Brown, M. J., Mills, D. A., Kramer, P. D., Stüve, O., & Frohman, E. M. (2008). Modeling Uhthoff's phenomenon in MS patients with internuclear ophthalmoparesis. Neurology, 70, 1098-1106.

Davis, T. S., Parker, R. A., House, P. A., Bagley, E., Wendelken, S., Normann, R. A., & Greger, B. (2012). Spatial and temporal characteristics of V1 microstimulation during chronic implantation of a microelectrode array in a behaving macaque. Journal of Neural Engineering, 9, 065003 doi:10.1088/1741-2560/9/6/065003.

Davis, T. S., Torab, K., House, P., & Greger, B. (2009). A minimally invasive approach to long-term head fixation in behaving nonhuman primates. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 181, 106-110.

Day, R.-F. (2010). Examining the validity of the Needleman-Wunsch algorithm in identifying decision strategy with eye-movement data. Decision Support Systems, 49, 396-403.

Day, R.-F., Lin, C.-H., Huang, W.-H., & Chuang, S.-H. (2009). Effects of music tempo and task difficulty on multi-attribute decision-making: An eye-tracking approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 130-143.

Day, R.-F., Shyi, G. C.-W., & Wang, J.-C. (2006). The effect of flash banners on multiattribute decision making: Distractor or source of arousal? Psychology and Marketing, 23, 369-382.

De Filippo, C. L., & Lansing, C. R. (2006). Eye fixations of deaf and hearing observers in simultaneous communication perception. Ear & Hearing, 27, 331-352.

De Grave, D. D. J., & Bruno, N. (2010). The effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccades is modulated by spatial predictability and saccadic latency. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 671-679.

De Grave, D. D. J., Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). The influence of the Brentano illusion on eye and hand movements. Journal of Vision, 6, 727-738. http://journalofvision.org/6/7/5/.

De Grave, D. D. J., Hesse, C., Brouwer, A.-M., & Franz, V. H. (2008). Fixation locations when grasping partly occluded objects. Journal of Vision, 8(7):5, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/8/7/5/, doi:10.1167/8.7.5.

De Grave, D. D. J., Smeets, J. B. J., & Brenner, E. (2006). Why are saccades influenced by the Brentano illusion? Experimental Brain Research, 175, 177-182.

de Hemptinne, C., Barnes, G. R., & Missal, M. (2010). Influence of previous target motion on anticipatory pursuit deceleration. Experimental Brain Research, 207, 173-184.

de Hemptinne, C., Ivanoiu, A., Lefèvre, P., & Missal, M. (2013). How does Parkinson’s disease and aging affect temporal expectation and the implicit timing of eye movements? Neuropsychologia, 51, 340-348.

De Luca, M., Pontillo, M., Primativo, S., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2013). The eye-voice lead during oral reading in developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:696. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00696.

De Lussanet, M. H. E., Fadiga, L., Michels, L., Seitz, R. J., Kleiser, R., & Lappe, M. (2008). Interaction of visual hemifield and body view in biological motion perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 514-522.

De Vries, J. P., Hooge, I. T. C., Wertheim, A. H., & Verstraten, F. A. J. (2013). Background, an important factor in visual search. Vision Research, 86, 128-138.

de Vries, J. P., Hooge, I. T. C., Wiering, M. A., & Verstraten, F. A. J. (2011). How longer saccade latencies lead to a competition for salience. Psychological Science, 22, 916-923.

de Vries, J. P., Hooge, I. T. C., Wiering, M. A., & Verstraten, F. A. J. (2011). Saccadic selection and crowding in visual search: stronger lateral masking leads to shorter search times. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 119-131.

Debono, K., Schütz, A. C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Illusory bending of a pursuit target. Vision Research,57, 51-60.

Debono, K., Schütz, A. C., Spering, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Receptive fields for smooth pursuit eye movements and motion perception. Vision Research, 50, 2729-2739.

Deffieux, T., Younan, Y., Wattiez, N., Tanter, M., Pouget, P., & Aubry, J. F. (2013). Low-intensity focused ultrasound modulates monkey visuomotor behavior. Current Biology, 23, 2430-2433.

Degani, A. M., Danna-Dos-Santos, A., Robert, T., & Latash, M. L. (2010). Kinematic synergies during saccades involving whole-body rotation: A study based on the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis. Human Movement Science, 29, 243-258.

Dell'Osso, L. F., & Jacobs, J. B. (2013). Normal pursuit-system limitations - First discovered in infantile nystagmus syndrome. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 6(1):2, 1-24.

Dell'Osso, L. F., Hertle, R. W., Leigh, R. J., Jacobs, J. B, King, S., & Yaniglos, S. (2011). Effects of topical brinzolamide on infantile nystagmus syndrome waveforms: Eyedrops for nystagmus. Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology, 31, 228-233.

Delogu, F., Vespignani, F., & Sanford, A. J. (2010). Effects of intensionality on sentence and discourse processing: Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 352-379.

Demichelis, A., Olivier, G., & Berthoz, A. (2013). Motor transfer from map ocular exploration to locomotion during spatial navigation from memory. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 605-611.

Desanghere, L., & Marotta, J. J. (2011). "Graspability" of objects affects gaze patterns during perception and action tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 177-187.

Desestret, V., Streichenberger, N., Panouillères, M., Pélisson, D., Plus, B., Duyckaerts, C., Burns, D. K., Scheiber, C., Vighetto, A., & Tilikete, C.. (2013). An elderly woman with difficulty reading and abnormal eye movements. Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology, 33, 296-301.

Desmet, T., & Gibson, E. (2003). Disambiguation preferences and corpus frequencies in noun phrase conjunction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 353-374.

Desmet, T., de Baecke, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). The influence of referential discourse context on modifier attachment in Dutch. Memory & Cognition, 30, 150-157.

Desmet, T., de Baecke, C., Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., & Vonk, W. (2006). Relative clause attachment in Dutch: On-line comprehension corresponds to corpus frequencies when lexical variables are taken into account. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 453-485.

Desmurget, M., Turner, R. S., Prablanc, C., Russo, G. S., Alexander, G. E., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). Updating target location at the end of an orienting saccade affects the characteristics of simple point-to-point movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 1510-1536.

Desrochers, T. M., Jin, D. Z., Goodman, N. D., & Graybiel, A. M. (2010). Optimal habits can develop spontaneously through sensitivity to local cost. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 20512-20517.

Dessing, J. C., Byrne, P. A., Abadeh, A., & Crawford, J. D. (2012). Hand-related rather than goal-related source of gaze-dependent errors in memory-guided reaching. Journal of Vision, 12(11):17, 1-8, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/11/17, doi:10.1167/12.11.17.

Dessing, J. C., Crawford, J. D., & Medendorp, W. P. (2011). Spatial updating across saccades during manual interception. Journal of Vision, 11(10):4, 1-18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/10/4, doi:10.1167/11.10.4.

Dessing, J. C., Rey, F. P., & Beek, P. J. (2012). Gaze fixation improves the stability of expert juggling. Experimental Brain Research, 216, 635-644.

Dessing, J. C., Wijdenes, L. O., Peper, C. E., & Beek, P. J. (2009). Visuomotor transformation for interception: catching while fixating. Experimental Brain Research, 196, 511-527.

Dessing, J.C., Vesia, M., & Crawford, J.D. (2013). The role of areas MT+/V5 and SPOC in spatial and temporal control of manual interception: an rTMS study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7:15. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00015.

Dettler, N., Strüber, D., Fründ, I., Schadow, J., Lenz, D., Rach, S., Körner, U., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Gamma in motion: Pattern reversal elicits stronger gamma-band responses than motion. NeuroImage, 55, 808-817.

Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (2003). Delayed saccades, but not delayed manual aiming movements, require visual attention shifts. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1004, 289-296.

Deutsch, A., & Bentin, S. (2001). Syntactic and semantic factors in processing gender agreement in Hebrew: Evidence from ERPs and eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 200-224.

Deutsch, A., & Rayner, K. (1999). Initial fixation location effects in reading Hebrew words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 393-421.

Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pelleg, S., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2003). Early morphological effects in reading: Evidence from parafoveal preview benefit in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 415-422.

Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2000). Early morphological effects in word recognition in Hebrew: Evidence from parafoveal preview benefit. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 487-506.

Devue, C., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Oculomotor guidance and capture by irrelevant faces. PLoS ONE 7(4): e34598. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034598.

Devue, C., Van der Stigchel, S., Brédart, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). You do not find your own face faster; you just look at it longer. Cognition, 111, 114-122.

Dhawan, S., Deubel, H., & Jonikaitis, D. (2013). Inhibition of saccades elicits attentional suppression. Journal of Vision, 13(6):9, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/6/9, doi:10.1167/13.6.9.

Di Stasi, L. L., Antolí, A., & Cañas, J. J. (2011). Main sequence: An index for detecting mental workload variation in complex tasks. Applied Ergonomics, 42, 807-813.

Di Stasi, L. L., Antolí, A., & Cañas, J. J. (2013). Evaluating mental workload while interacting with computer-generated artificial environments. Entertainment Computing, 4, 63-69.

Di Stasi, L. L., Antolí, A., Gea, M., & Cañas, J. J. (2011). A neuroergonomic approach to evaluating mental workload in hypermedia interactions. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 41, 298-304.

Di Stasi, L. L., Catena, A., Canas, J. J., Macknik, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2013). Saccadic velocity as an arousal index in naturalistic tasks. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 968-975.

Di Stasi, L. L., Contreras, D., Cándido, A., Cañas, J. J., & Catena, A. (2011). Behavioral and eye-movement measures to track improvements in driving skills of vulnerable road users: First-time motorcycle riders. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 14, 26-35.

Di Stasi, L. L., Marchitto, M., Antolí, A., Baccino, T., & Cañas, J. J. (2010). Approximation of on-line mental workload index in ATC simulated multitasks. Journal of Air Transport Management, 16, 330-333.

Di Stasi, L. L., McCamy, M. B., Catena, A., Macknik, S. L., Cañas, J. J., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2013). Microsaccade and drift dynamics reflect mental fatigue. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 2389-2398.

Di Stasi, L. L., Renner, R., Catena, A., Cañas, J. J., Velichkovsky, B. M., & Pannasch, S. (2012). Towards a driver fatigue test based on the saccadic main sequence: A partial validation by subjective report data. Transportation Research Part C: Emerging Technologies, 21, 122-133.

Di Stasi, L. L., Renner, R., Staehr, P., Helmert, J. R., Velichkovsky, B. M., Cañas, J. J., Catena, A., & Pannasch, S. (2010). Saccadic peak velocity sensitivity to variations in mental workload. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 81, 413-417.

Di Stasi, L. L., Álvarez-Valbuena, V., Cañas, J. J., Maldonado, A., Catena, A., Antolí, A., & Candido, A. (2009). Risk behaviour and mental workload: Multimodal assessment techniques applied to motorbike riding simulation. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 12, 361-370.

DiPaola, S., Riebe, C., & Enns, J. T. (2010). Rembrandt's textural agency: A shared perspective in visual art and science. Leonardo, 43, 145-151.

DiPaola, S., Riebe, C., & Enns, J. T. (2013). Following the masters: Portrait viewing and appreciation is guided by selective detail. Perception, 42, 608-630.

Dias, J. C., Sajda, P., Dmochowski, J. P., & Parra, L. C. (2013). EEG precursors of detected and missed targets during free-viewing search. Journal of Vision, 13(13):13, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/13/13, doi:10.1167/13.13.13.

Dickinson, C. A., & Intraub, H. (2008). Transsaccadic representation of layout: What is the time course of boundary extension? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 543-555.

Dickinson, C. A., & Intraub, H. (2009). Spatial asymmetries in viewing and remembering scenes: Consequences of an attentional bias? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 1251-1262.

Dickinson, C. A., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2005). Marking rejected distractors: A gaze-contingent technique for measuring memory during search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1120-1126.

Dickinson, C. A., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2007). Memory for the search path: Evidence for a high-capacity representation of search history. Vision Research, 47, 1745-1755.

Dickinson, C.A., & Zelinsky, G.J. (2013). New evidence for strategic differences between static and dynamic search tasks: an individual observer analysis of eye movements. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00008.

Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2007). Modeling spatial effects in visual-tactile saccadic reaction time. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 56-67.

Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2007). Why two "distractors" are better than one: Modeling the effect of non-target auditory and tactile stimuli on visual saccadic reaction time. Experimental Brain Research, 179, 43-54.

Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2008). Crossmodal interaction in saccadic reaction time: separating multisensory from warning effects in the time window of integration model. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 1-22.

Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2008). When a high-intensity "distractor" is better then a low-intensity one: Modeling the effect of an auditory or tactile nontarget stimulus on visual saccadic reaction time. Brain Research, 1242, 219-230.

Diederich, A., Colonius, H., & Schomburg, A. (2008). Assessing age-related multisensory enhancement with the time-window-of-integration model. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2556-2562.

Diederich, A., Colonius, H., Bockhorst, D., & Tabeling, S. (2003). Visual-tactile spatial interaction in saccade generation. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 328-337.

Diederich, A., Schomburg, A., & Colonius, H. (2012). Saccadic reaction times to audiovisual stimuli show effects of oscillatory phase reset. PLoS ONE 7(10): e44910. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044910.

Diehl, M. D., & Pidcoe, P. E. (2010). The Influence of gaze stabilization and fixation on stepping reactions in younger and older adults. Journal of Geriatric Physical Therapy, 33, 19-25.

Digirolamo, G. J., McCarley, J. S., Kramer, A. F., & Griffin, H. J. (2008). Voluntary and reflexive eye movements to illusory lengths. Visual Cognition, 16, 68-89.

Dijkerman, H. C., McIntosh, R. D., Milner, A. D., Rossetti, Y. Tilikete, C., & Roberts, R. C. (2003). Ocular scanning and perceptual size distortion in hemispatial neglect: Effects of prism adaptation and sequential stimulus presentation. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 220-230.

Dillon, B., Mishler, A., Sloggett, S., & Phillips, C. (2013). Contrasting intrusion profiles for agreement and anaphora: Experimental and modeling evidence. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 85-103.

Dixson, A. F., & Dixson, B. J. (2012). Venus figurines of the European paleolithic: Symbols of fertility or attractiveness? Journal of Anthropology, vol. 2011, Article ID 569120, 11 pages, 2011. doi:10.1155/2011/569120.

Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Linklater, W. L., & Dixson, A. F. (2010). Watching the hourglass - eye tracking reveals men's appreciation of the female form. Human Nature, 21, 355-370.

Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Linklater, W. L., & Dixson, A. F. (2011). Eye Tracking of men's preferences for female breast size and areola pigmentation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 51-58.

Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Linklater, W. L., & Dixson, A. F. (2011). Eye-tracking of men's preferences for waist-to-hip ratio and breast size of women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 43-50.

Dodd, M. D., Balzer, A., Jacobs, C., Gruszczynski, M., Smith, K. B., & Hibbing, J. R. (2012). The political left rolls with the good, the political right confronts: Physiology and Cognition in Politics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B., 367, 640 - 649.

Dodd, M. D., Van der Stigchel, S., & Hollingworth, A. (2009). Novelty is not always the best policy: Inhibition of return and facilitation of return as a function of visual task. Psychological Science, 20, 333-339.

Dombrowe, I., Donk, M., & Olivers, C. N. L. (2011). The costs of switching attentional sets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 2481-2488.

Dombrowe, I., Donk, M., Wright, H., Olivers, C. N., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). The contribution of stimulus-driven and goal-driven mechanisms to feature-based selection in patients with spatial attention deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29, 249-274.

Donk, M., & Soesman, L. (2010). Salience is only briefly represented: Evidence from probe-detection performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 36, 286-302.

Donk, M., & van Zoest, W. (2008). Effects of saliency are short-lived. Psychological Science, 19, 733-739.

Donk, M., & van Zoest, W. (2011). No control in orientation search: The effects of instruction on oculomotor selection in visual search. Vision Research, 51, 2156-2166.

Donnelly, N., Cornes, K., & Menneer, T. (2012). An examination of the processing capacity of features in the Thatcher illusion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1475-1487.

Donovan, T., Crawford, T. J., & Litchfield, D. (2012). Negative priming for target selection with saccadic eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 483-494.

Dorr, M., & Bex, P. J. (2013).Peri-saccadic natural vision. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1211-1217.

Dorr, M., Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Barth, E. (2009). The contribution of low-level features at the centre of gaze to saccade target selection. Vision Research, 49, 2918-2926.

Dorr, M., Martinetz, T., Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Barth, E. (2010). Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 10(10):28, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/10/28, doi:10.1167/10.10.28.

Dorr, M., Vig, E., & Barth, E. (2012). Eye movement prediction and variability on natural video data sets. Visual Cognition, 20, 495-514.

Doyle, M. C., & Walker, R. (2002). Multisensory interactions in saccade target selection: Curved saccade trajectories. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 116-130.

Doyle, M., & Walker, R. (2001). Curved saccade trajectories: Voluntary and reflexive saccades curve away from irrelevant distractors. Experimental Brain Research, 139, 333-344.

Drew, T., Cunningham, C., & Wolfe, J. M. (2012). When and why might a computer-aided detection (CAD) system interfere with visual search? An eye-tracking study. Academic Radiology, 19, 1260-1267.

Drew, T., Vo, M. L.-H., Olwal, A., Jacobson, F., Seltzer, S. E., & Wolfe, J. M. (2013). Scanners and drillers: Characterizing expert visual search through volumetric images. Journal of Vision, 13(10):3, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/10/3, doi:10.1167/13.10.3.

Drewes, J., Trommershäuser, J., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Parallel visual search and rapid animal detection in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 11(2):20, 1-21, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/2/20, doi:10.1167/11.2.20.

Drieghe, D. (2008). Foveal processing and word skipping during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 856-860.

Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., & Desmet, T. (2005). Parafoveal-on-foveal effects on eye movements in text reading: Does an extra space make a difference? Vision Research, 45, 1693-1706.

Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., Desmet, T., & De Baecke, C. (2004). Word skipping in reading: On the interplay of linguistic and visual factors. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 79-103.

Drieghe, D., Desmet, T., & Brysbaert, M. (2007). How important are linguistic factors in word skipping during reading? British Journal of Psychology, 98, 157-171.

Drieghe, D., Pollatsek, A., Juhasz, B. J., & Rayner, K. (2010). Parafoveal processing during reading is reduced across a morphological boundary. Cognition, 116, 136-142.

Droll, J. A., Abbey, C. K., & Eckstein, M. P. (2009). Learning cue validity through performance feedback. Journal of Vision, 9(2):18, 1-22, http://journalofvision.org/9/2/18/, doi:10.1167/9.2.18.

Droll, J. A., Gigone, K., & Hayhoe, M. M. (2007). Learning where to direct gaze during change detection. Journal of Vision, 7(14):6, 1-12, http://journalofvision.org/7/14/6/, doi:10.1167/7.14.6.

Du, F., Qi, Y., Li, X., & Zhang, K. (2013). Dual processes of oculomotor capture by abrupt onset: Rapid involuntary capture and sluggish voluntary prioritization. PLoS ONE 8(11): e80678. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080678.

Duchesne, J., Bouvier, V., Guillemé, J., & Coubard, O. A. (2012). Maxwellian eye fixation during natural scene perception. The Scientific World Journal, vol. 2012, Article ID 956340, 12 pages, 2012. doi:10.1100/2012/956340.

Ducrot, S., Pynte, J., Ghio, A., & Lété, B. (2013). Visual and linguistic determinants of the eyes' initial fixation position in reading development. Acta Psychologica, 142, 287-298.

Dudschig, C., Souman, J., Lachmair, M., Vega, Idl., & Kaup, B. (2013). Reading "sun" and looking up: The influence of language on saccadic eye movements in the vertical dimension. PLoS ONE 8(2): e56872. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056872.

Duijnhouwer, J., Krekelberg, B., van den Berg, A., & van Wezel, R. (2010). Temporal integration of focus position signal during compensation for pursuit in optic flow. Journal of Vision, 10(14):14, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/14, doi:10.1167/10.14.14.

Duijnhouwer, J., van Wezel, R. J. A., & van den Berg, A. V. (2008). The role of motion capture in an illusory transformation of optic flow fields. Journal of Vision, 8(4):27, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/8/4/27/, doi:10.1167/8.4.27.

Duka, T., Crombag, H. S., Stephens, D. N. (2011). Experimental medicine in drug addiction: towards behavioral, cognitive and neurobiological biomarkers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25, 1235-1255.

Dukewich, K. R., Klein, R. M., & Christie, J. (2008). The effect of gaze on gaze direction while looking at art. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1141-1147.

Dumitru, M. L., Joergensen, G. H., Cruickshank, A. G., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2013). Language-guided visual processing affects reasoning: The role of referential and spatial anchoring. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 562-571.

Durgin, F. H., Doyle, E., & Egan, L. (2008). Upper-left gaze bias reveals competing search strategies in a reverse Stroop task. Acta Psychologica, 127, 428-448.

Dussias, P. E. (2004). Parsing a first language like a second: The erosion of L1 parsing strategies in Spanish-English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 355-371.

Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 101-116.

Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 353-387.

Duyck, W., Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2007). Visual word recognition by bilinguals in a sentence context: Evidence for nonselective lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 663-679.

Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., & Carreiras, M. (2008). NoA's ark: Influence of the number of associates in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1072-1077.

Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M. (2009). Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: Evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition, 110, 284-292.

Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2009). Eye movements when reading words with $YMßOL$ and NUM83R5: There is a cost. Visual Cognition, 17, 617-631.

Duñabeitia, J.A., Dimitropoulou, M., Estévez, A., & Carreiras, M. (2013). The influence of reading expertise in mirror-letter perception: Evidence from beginning and expert readers. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7, 124-135.

Dépatie, L., O'Driscoll, G. A., Holahan, A.-L. V., Atkinson, V., Thavundayi, J. X., Kin, N. Y., & La, S. (2002). Nicotine and behavioral markers of risk for schizophrenia: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 1056-1070.

Back To Top

E

Eagleman, S. L., & Dragoi, V. (2012). Image sequence reactivation in awake V4 networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 19450-19455.

Ebitz, R. B., Watson, K. K., & Platt, M. L. (2013). Oxytocin blunts social vigilance in the rhesus macaque. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 11630-11635.

Eckstein, M. P., Beutter, B. R., Pham, B. T., Shimozaki, S. S., & Stone, L. S. (2007). Similar neural representations of the target for saccades and perception during search. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1266-1270.

Eckstein, M. P., Drescher, B. A., & Shimozaki, S. S. (2006). Attentional cues in real scenes, saccadic targeting, and bayesian priors. Psychological Science, 17, 973-980.

Eckstein, M. P., Mack, S. C., Liston, D. B., Bogush, L., Menzel, R., & Krauzlis, R. J. (2013). Rethinking human visual attention: Spatial cueing effects and optimality of decisions by honeybees, monkeys and humans. Vision Research, 85, 5-19.

Edelman, J. A., & Xu, K. Z. (2009). Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 1222-1234.

Edelman, J. A., Kristjánsson, Á., & Nakayama, K. (2007). The influence of object-relative visuomotor set on express saccades. Journal of Vision, 7(6):12, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/7/6/12/, doi:10.1167/7.6.12.

Edelman, J. A., Valenzuela, N., & Barton, J. S. (2006). Antisaccade velocity, but not latency, results from a lack of saccade visual guidance. Vision Research, 46, 1411-1421.

Egaña, J.I., Devia, C., Mayol, R., Parrini, J., Orellana, G., Ruiz, A., & Maldonado, P.E. (2013). Small saccades and image complexity during free viewing of natural images in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4:37. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00037.

Ego, C., Orban de Xivry, J.-J., Nassogne, M.-C., Yüksel, D., & Lefèvre, P. (2013). The saccadic system does not compensate for the immaturity of the smooth pursuit system during visual tracking in children. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 358-367.

Ehresman, C., Saucier, D., Heath, M., & Binsted, G. (2008). Online corrections can produce illusory bias during closed-loop pointing. Experimental Brain Research, 188, 371-378.

Einhäuser, W., Koch, C., & Carter, O. (2010). Pupil dilation betrays the timing of decisions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4:18. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00018

Einhäuser, W., Kruse, W., Hoffmann, K., & König, P. (2006). Differences of monkey and human overt attention under natural conditions. Vision Research, 46, 1194-1209.

Einhäuser, W., Rutishauser, U., & Koch, C. (2008). Task-demands can immediately reverse the effects of sensory-driven saliency in complex visual stimuli. Journal of Vision, 8(2):2, 1-19, http://journalofvision.org/8/2/2/, doi:10.1167/8.2.2.

Einhäuser, W., Rutishauser, U., Frady, E. P., Nadler, S., König, P., & Koch, C. (2006). The relation of phase noise and luminance contrast to overt attention in complex visual stimuli. Journal of Vision, 6, 1148-1158. http://journalofvision.org/6/11/1/1148.

Einhäuser, W., Spain, M., & Perona, P. (2008). Objects predict fixations better than early saliency. Journal of Vision, 8(14):18, 1-26, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/18/, doi:10.1167/8.14.18.

Einhäuser, W., Stout, J., Koch, C., & Carter, O. (2008). Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1704-1709.

Eiter, B. M., & Inhoff, A. W. (2010). Visual word recognition during reading is followed by subvocal articulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 36, 457-470.

Elahipanah, A., Christensen, B. K., & Reingold, E. M. (2011). Attentional guidance during visual search among patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 131, 224-230.

Elahipanah, A., Christensen, B. K., & Reingold, E. M. (2011). Controlling the Spotlight of Attention: Visual Span Size and Flexibility in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3370-3376.

Elahipanah, A., Christensen, B. K., & Reingold, E. M. (2011). What can eye movements tell us about Symbol Digit substitution by patients with schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Research, 127, 137-143.

Ellenbuerger, T., Boutin, A., Panzer, S., Blandin, Y., Fischer, L., Schorer, J., & Shea, C. H. (2012). Observational training in visual half-fields and the coding of movement sequences. Human Movement Science, 31, 1436-1448.

Ellert, M., & Holler, A. (2011). Semantic and Structural Constraints on the Resolution of Ambiguous Personal Pronouns - A Psycholinguistic Study. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 7099, 157-170.

Ellis, J. J., Glaholt, M. G., & Reingold, E. M. (2011). Eye movements reveal solution knowledge prior to insight. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 768-776.

Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2010). Learned attention effects in L2 temporal reference: The first hour and the next eight semesters. Language Learning, 60, 85-108.

Emeric, E. E., Brown, J. W., Boucher, L., Carpenter, R. H. S., Hanes, D. P., Harris, R., Logan, G. d., Mashru, R. N., Paré, M., Pouget, P., Stuphorn, V., Taylor, T. L., & Schall, J. D. (2007). Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys. Vision Research, 47, 35-49.

Emerich, S. M., Ruppel, J. D. N., Al-Aidroos, N., Pratt, J., & Ferber, S. (2008). Out with the old: Inhibition of old items in a preview search is limited. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1552-1557.

Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43, 1035-1045.

Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2004). Microsaccades keep the eyes' balance. Psychological Science, 15, 431-436.

Engbert, R., & Krügel, A. (2010). Readers use bayesian estimation for eye movement control. Psychological Science, 21, 366-371.

Engbert, R., & Mergenthaler, K. (2006). Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 18, 7192-7197.

Engbert, R., & Nuthmann, A. (2008). Self-consistent estimation of mislocated fixations during reading. PLoS ONE 3(2): e1534. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001534.

Engelhardt, P. E., Ferreira, F., & Patsenko, E. G. (2010). Pupillometry reveals processing load during spoken language comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 639-645.

Engmann, S., 'T Hart, B. M., Sieren, T., Onat, S., König, P., & Einhäuser, W. (2009). Saliency on a natural scene background: Effects of color and luminance contrast add linearly. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1337-1352.

Enns, J. T., & MacDonald, S. C. (2013). The role of clarity and blur in guiding visual attention in photographs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 568-578.

Erkelens, C. J. (2006). Coordination of smooth pursuit and saccades. Vision Research, 46, 163-170.

Escudero, P., Hayes-Harb, R., & Mitterer, H. (2008). Novel second-language words and asymmetric lexical access. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 345-360.

Essig, K., Pomplun, M., & Ritter, H. (2006). A neural network for 3D gaze recording with binocular eye trackers. International Journal of Parallel, Emergent, and Distributed Systems, 21, 79-95.

Etchells, P. J., Benton, C. P., Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2010). The target velocity integration function for saccades. Journal of Vision, 10(6):7, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/7, doi:10.1167/10.6.7.

Etchells, P. J., Benton, C. P., Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2011). Testing a simplified method for measuring velocity integration in saccades using a manipulation of target contrast. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00115.

Evans, K., Rotello, C., Li, X., & Rayner, K. (2009). Scene perception and memory revealed by eye movements and receiver-operating characteristic analyses: Does a cultural difference truly exist? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 276-285.

Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2005). What children are looking at during shared storybook reading: Evidence from eye movement monitoring. Psychological Science, 16, 913-920.

Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2013). Vocabulary acquisition without adult explanations in repeated shared book reading: An eye movement study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 596-608.

Evans, M. A., Saint-Aubin, J., & Landry, N. (2009). Letter names and alphabet book reading by senior kindergarteners: An eye movement study. Child Development, 80, 1824-1841.

Evans, W. S., Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (2011). Effects of concurrent arithmetical and syntactic complexity on self-paced reaction times and eye fixations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1203-1211.

Evens, D. R., & Ludwig, C. J. H. (2010). Dual-task costs and benefits in anti-saccade performance. Experimental Brain Research, 205, 545-557.

Everdell, I. T., Marsh, H., Yurick, M. D., Munhall, K. G., & Paré, M. (2007). Gaze behaviour in audiovisual speech perception: Asymmetrical distribution of face-directed fixations. Perception, 36, 1535-1545.

Ezzati, A., Golzar, A., & Afraz, A. S. R. (2008). Topography of the motion aftereffect with and without eye movements. Journal of Vision, 8(14):23, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/23/, doi:10.1167/8.14.23.

Back To Top

F

Fairhall, S. J., Dickson, C. A., Scott, L., & Pearce, P. C. (2006). A non-invasive method for studying an index of pupil diameter and visual performance in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Medical Primatology, 35, 67-77.

Faivre, N., & Kouider, S. (2011). Increased sensory evidence reverses nonconscious priming during crowding. Journal of Vision, 11(13):16, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/13/16, doi:10.1167/11.13.16.

Faivre, N., & Kouider, S. (2011). Multi-feature objects elicit nonconscious priming despite crowding. Journal of Vision, 11(3):2, 1-10, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/3/2, doi:10.1167/11.3.2.

Faivre, N., Berthet, V., & Kouider, S. (2012). Nonconscious influences from emotional faces: A comparison of visual crowding, masking, and continuous flash suppression. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 129. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00129.

Faivre, N., Charron, S., Roux, P., Lehéricy, S., & Kouider, S. (2012). Nonconscious emotional processing involves distinct neural pathways for pictures and videos. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3736-3744.

Farrell, S., Ludwig, C. J. H., Ellis, L. A., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2010). Influence of environmental statistics on inhibition of saccadic return. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 929-934.

Farris-Trimble, A., & McMurray, b. (2013). Test-retest reliability of eye tracking in the visual world paradigm for the study of real-time spoken word recognition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 1328-1345.

Featherstone, C. R., & Sturt, P. (2010). Because there was a cause for concern: An investigation into a word-specific prediction account of the implicit-causality effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 3-15.

Fecteau, J. H., Au, C., Armstrong, I. T., & Munoz, D. P. (2004). Sensory biases produce the alternation advantage found in sequential saccadic eye movement tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 84-91.

Fekete, T., Beacher, F. D. C. C., Cha, J., Rubin, D., Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2014). Small-world network properties in prefrontal cortex correlate with predictors of psychopathology risk in young children: A NIRS study. NeuroImage, 85, 345-353.

Felius, J., & Muhanna, Z. A. (2013). Visual deprivation and foveation characteristics both underlie visual acuity deficits in idiopathic infantile nystagmus. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54,3520-3525.

Felius, J., Fu, V. L. N., Birch, E. E., Hertle, R. W., Jost, R. M., & Subramanian, V. (2011). Quantifying nystagmus in infants and young children: Relation between foveation and visual acuity deficit. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science. 52, 8724-8731.

Felser, C., & Cunnings, I. (2012). Processing reflexives in a second language: The timing of structural and discourse-level constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 571-603.

Felser, C., Cunnings, I., Batterham, C., & Clahsen, H. (2012). The timing of island effects in nonnative sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 67-98.

Felser, C., Sato, M., & Bertenshaw, N. (2009). The on-line application of binding Principle A in English as a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 485-502.

Feng, G. (2006). Eye movements as time-series random variables: A stochastic model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 70-95.

Feng, G. (2009). Mixed responses: Why readers spend less time at unfavorable landing positions. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(2):2, 1-26.

Feng, G. (2012). Is there a common control mechanism for anti-saccades and reading eye movements? Evidence from distributional analyses. Vision Research, 57, 35-50.

Feng, G., Miller, K., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2001). Rowed to recovery: The use of phonological and orthographic information in reading Chinese and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 1079-1100.

Feng, G., Miller, K., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2009). Orthography and the development of reading processes: An eye-movement study of Chinese and English. Child Development, 80, 720-735.

Ferber, S., & Murray L. (2005). Are perceptual judgments dissociated from motor processes? A prism adaptation study. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 453-456.

Ferguson, H. J. (2012). Eye movements reveal rapid concurrent access to factual and counterfactual interpretations of the world. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 939-961.

Ferguson, H. J., Scheepers, C., & Sanford, A. J. (2010). Expectations in counterfactual and theory of mind reasoning. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 297-346.

Fernández-Martín, A., Gutiérrez-García, A., & Calvo, M. G. (2013). A smile radiates outwards and biases the eye expression. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, E53-E63.

Ferreira, F., Foucart, A., & Engelhardt, P. E. (2013). Language processing in the visual world: Effects of preview, visual complexity, and prediction. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 165-182.

Ferri, J., Schmidt, J., Hajcak, G., & Canli, T. (2013). Neural correlates of attentional deployment within unpleasant pictures. NeuroImage, 70, 268-277.

Festman, Y., Adam, J. J., Pratt, J., & Fischer, M. H. (2013).Continuous hand movement induces a far-hand bias in attentional priority. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 644-649.

Fiebelkorn, I. C., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Mercier, M. R., Snyder, A. C., & Molholm, S. (2011). Ready, set, reset: Stimulus-locked periodicity in behavioral performance demonstrates the consequences of cross-sensory phase reset. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9971-9981.

Fiebelkorn, I. C., Snyder, A. C., Mercier, M. R., Butler, J. S., Molholm, S., & Foxe, J. J. (2013). Cortical cross-frequency coupling predicts perceptual outcomes. NeuroImage, 69, 126-137.

Fiehler, K., Schütz, I., & Henriques, D. Y. P. (2011). Gaze-centered spatial updating of reach targets across different memory delays. Vision Research, 51, 890-897.

Field, D. T., Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2007). Neural systems in the visual control of steering. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8002-8010.

Filik, R., & Barber, E. (2011). Inner speech during silent reading reflects the reader's regional accent. PLoS ONE 6(10): e25782. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025782.

Filik, R., & Leuthold, H. (2013). The role of character-based knowledge in online narrative comprehension: Evidence from eye movements and ERPs. Brain Research, 1506. 94-104.

Filik, R., & Moxey, L. M. (2010). The on-line processing of written irony. Cognition, 116, 421-436.

Fiorillo, C. D. (2011). Transient activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by reward risk. Neuroscience, 197, 162-171.

Fiorillo, C. D., Yun, S. R., & Song, M. R. (2013). Diversity and homogeneity in responses of midbrain dopamine neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4693-4709.

Fiorillo, C. D., Yun, S. R., & Song, M. R. (2013). Multiphasic temporal dynamics in responses of midbrain dopamine neurons to appetitive and aversive stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4710-4725.

Firestone, A., Turk-Browne, N. B., & Ryan, J. D. (2007). Age-related deficits in face recognition are related to underlying changes in scanning behavior. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 594-607.

Fischer, T., Graupner, S.-T., Velichkovsky, B.M., & Pannasch, S. (2013). Attentional dynamics during free picture viewing: Evidence from oculomotor behavior and electrocortical activity. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:17. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00017.

Fitzsimmons, G., & Drieghe, D. (2011). The influence of number of syllables on word skipping during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 736-741.

Fitzsimmons, G., & Drieghe, D. (2013). How fast can predictability influence word skipping during reading? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1054-1063.

Fleming, K. K., Bandy, C. L., & Kimble, M. O. (2010). Decisions to shoot in a weapon identification task: The influence of cultural stereotypes and perceived threat on false positive errors. Social Neuroscience, 5, 201-220.

Flowe, H. (2011). An exploration of visual behaviour in eyewitness identification tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 244-254.

Flowe, H. D., & Cottrell, G. (2011). An examination of simultaneous lineup decision processes using eye tracking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 443-451.

Flowe, H.D., Hope, L., Hillstrom, & A.P. (2013). Oculomotor examination of the weapon focus effect: Does a gun automatically engage visual attention? PLoS ONE 8(12): e81011. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0081011.

Foraker, S., & McElree, B. (2007). The role of prominence in pronoun resolution: Active versus passive representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 357-383.

Foraker, S., & Murphy, G. L. (2012). Polysemy in sentence comprehension: Effects of meaning dominance. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 407-425.

Ford, K. A., & Everling, S. (2009). Neural activity in primate caudate nucleus associated with pro- and anti-saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 2334-2341.

Fornos, A. P., Sommerhalder, J., & Pelizzone, M. (2011). Reading with a simulated 60-channel implant. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5:57. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00057.

Fornos, A. P., Sommerhalder, J., Pittard, A., Safran, A. B., & Pelizzone, M. (2008). Simulation of artificial vision: IV. Visual information required to achieve simple pointing and manipulation tasks. Vision Research, 48, 1705-1718.

Fornos, A. P., Sommerhalder, J., Rappaz, B., Pelizzone, M., & Safran, A. B. (2006). Processes involved in oculomotor adaptation to eccentric reading. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 47, 1439-1447.

Fornos, A. P., Sommerhalder, J., Rappaz, B., Safran, A. B., & Pelizzone, M. (2005). Simulation of artificial vision, III: Do the spatial or temporal characteristics of stimulus pixelization really matter? Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 46, 3906-3912.

Forti, S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Sensitivity to object viewpoint and action instructions during search for targets in the lower visual field. Psychological Science, 19, 42-48.

Foster, T. E., Ardoin, S. P, & Binder, K. S. (2013). Underlying changes in repeated reading: An Eye Movement Study. School Psychology Review, 42, 140-156.

Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Asymmetries in the direction of saccades during perception of scenes and fractals: Effects of image type and image features. Vision Research, 50, 779-795.

Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Modelling the influence of central and peripheral information on saccade biases in gaze-contingent scene viewing. Visual Cognition, 20, 546-579.

Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Fixation-dependent memory for natural scenes: An experimental test of scanpath theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 41-56.

Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Optimal and preferred eye landing positions in objects and scenes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1707-1728.

Foulsham, T., & Sanderson, L. A. (2013). Look who's talking? Sound changes gaze behaviour in a dynamic social scene. Visual Cognition, 21, 922-944.

Foulsham, T., & Underwood, G. (2007). How does the purpose of inspection influence the potency of visual salience in scene perception? Perception, 36, 1123-1138.

Foulsham, T., & Underwood, G. (2008). What can saliency models predict about eye movements? Spatial and sequential aspects of fixations during encoding and recognition. Journal of Vision, 8(2):6, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/8/2/6/, doi:10.1167/8.2.6.

Foulsham, T., & Underwood, G. (2009). Does conspicuity enhance distraction? Saliency and eye landing position when searching for objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1088-1098.

Foulsham, T., & Underwood, G. (2011). If visual saliency predicts search, then why? Evidence from normal and gaze-contingent search tasks in natural scenes. Cognitive Computation, 3, 48-63.

Foulsham, T., Alan, R., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Scrambled eyes? Disrupting scene structure impedes focal processing and increases bottom-up guidance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 2008-2025.

Foulsham, T., Barton, J. J. S., Kingstone, A., Dewhurst, R., & Underwood, G. (2009). Fixation and saliency during search of natural scenes: the case of visual agnosia. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1994-2003.

Foulsham, T., Barton, J. J. S., Kingstone, A., Dewhurst, R., & Underwood, G. (2011). Modeling eye movements in visual agnosia with a saliency map approach: Bottom-up guidance or top-down strategy? Neural Networks, 24, 665-677.

Foulsham, T., Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Henrich, J., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Gaze allocation in a dynamic situation: Effects of social status and speaking. Cognition, 117, 319-331.

Foulsham, T., Farley, J., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Mind wandering in sentence reading: Decoupling the link between mind and eye. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 51-59.

Foulsham, T., Gray, A., Nasiopoulos, E., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Leftward biases in picture scanning and line bisection: A gaze-contingent window study. Vision Research, 78, 14-25.

Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Underwood, G. (2008). Turning the world around: Patterns in saccade direction vary with picture orientation. Vision Research, 48, 1777-1790.

Foulsham, T., Teszka, R., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Saccade control in natural images is shaped by the information visible at fixation: evidence from asymmetric gaze-contingent windows. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 266-283.

Foulsham, T., Walker, E., & Kingstone, A. (2011). The where, what and when of gaze allocation in the lab and the natural environment. Vision Research, 51, 1920-1931.

Fracasso, A., Caramazza, A., & Melcher, D. (2010). Continuous perception of motion and shape across saccadic eye movements. Journal of Vision, 10(13):14, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/13/14, doi:10.1167/10.13.14.

Franconeri, S. L., & Simons, D. J. (2005). The dynamic events that capture visual attention: A reply to Abrams & Christ (2005). Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 962-966.

Franconeri, S. L., Scimeca, J. M., Roth, J. C., Helseth, S. A., & Kahn, L. E. (2012). Flexible visual processing of spatial relationships. Cognition, 122, 210-227.

Franconeri, S. L., Simons, D. J., & Junge, J. A. (2004). Searching for stimulus-driven shifts of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 876-881.

Frank, S. L., Monsalve, I. F., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G. (2013). Reading time data for evaluating broad-coverage models of English sentence processing. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1182-1190.

Freeman, J., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2011). Metamers of the ventral stream. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 1195-1201.

Freeman, J., Brouwer, G. J., Heeger, D. J., & Merriam, E. P. (2011). Orientation decoding depends on maps, not columns. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4792-4804.

Freeman, J., Heeger, D. J., & Merriam, E. P. (2013). Coarse-scale biases for spirals and orientation in human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 19695-19703.

Freeman, T. C. A., Champion, R. A., & Warren, P. A. (2010). A Bayesian model of perceived head-centered velocity during smooth pursuit eye movement. Current Biology, 20, 757-762.

Freeman, T. C. A., Champion, R. A., Sumnall, J. H., & Snowden, R. J. (2009). Do we have direct access to retinal image motion during smooth pursuit eye movements? Journal of Vision, 9(1):33, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/9/1/33/, doi:10.1167/9.1.33.

Freeth, M., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013). What affects social attention? Social presence, eye contact and autistic traits. PLoS ONE 8(1): e53286. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053286.

Frenck-Mestre, C., Zardan, N., Colas, A., & Ghio, A. (2010). Eye-movement patterns of readers with down syndrome during sentence-processing: An exploratory study. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 115, 193-206.

Frens, M. A., & van der Geest, J. N. (2002). Scleral search coils influence saccade dynamics. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 692-698.

Frey, A., Ionescu, G., Lemaire, B., López-Orozco, F., Baccino, T., & Guérin-Dugué, A. (2013). Decision-making in information seeking on texts: an eye-fixation-related potentials investigation. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:39. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00039.

Frey, H. P., Molholm, S., Lalor, E. C., Russo, N. N., & Foxe, J. J. (2013). Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 2125-2138.

Frey, H.-P., Honey, C., & König, P. (2008). What's color got to do with it? The influence of color on visual attention in different categories. Journal of Vision, 8(14):6, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/6/, doi:10.1167/8.14.6.

Frey, H.-P., Kelly, S. P., Lalor, E. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2010). Early spatial attentional modulation of inputs to the fovea. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4547-4551.

Frey, H.-P., Wirz, K., Willenbockel, V., Betz, T., Schreiber, C., Troscianko, T., & König, P. (2011). Beyond correlation: do color features influence attention in rainforest? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5:36. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00036

Frey, J., & Ringach, D. L. (2011). Binocular eye movements evoked by self-induced motion parallax. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 17069-17073.

Frischen, A., & Tipper, S., P. (2004). Orienting attention via observed gaze shift evokes longer term inhibitory effects: Implications for social interactions, attention, and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 516-533.

Frisson, S., & McElree, B. (2008). Complement coercion is not modulated by competition: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1-11.

Frisson, S., & Wakefield, M. (2012). Psychological essentialist reasoning and perspective taking during reading: A donkey is not a zebra, but a plate can be a clock. Memory & Cognition, 40, 297-310.

Frisson, S., Niswander-Klement, E., & Pollatsek, A. (2008). The role of semantic transparency in the processing of English compound words. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 87-107.

Frohman, E. M., Frohman, T. C., Fleckenstein, J., Rackea, M. K., Hawker, K., & Kramer, P. D., (2001). Ocular contrapulsion in multiple sclerosis: clinical features and pathophysiological mechanisms. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 70, 688-692.

Frohman, E. M., Frohman, T. C., O'Suilleabhain, P., Zhang, H., Hawker, K., Racke, M. K., Frawley, W., Phillips, J. T., & Kramer, P. D. (2002). Quantitative oculographic characterisation of internuclear ophthalmoparesis in multiple sclerosis: The versional dysconjugacy index Z score. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 73, 51-55.

Frohman, E. M., O'Suilleabhain, P., Dewey, R. B., Jr., Frohman, T. C., & Kramer, P. D. (2003). A new measure of dysconjugacy in INO: The first-pass amplitude. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 210, 65-71.

Frohman, E. M., Zhang, H., Kramer, P. D., Fleckenstein, J., Hawker, K., Racke, M. K., & Frohman, T. C. (2001). MRI characteristics of the MLF in MS patients with chronic internuclear ophthalmoparesis. Neurology, 57, 762-768.

Frohman, T. C., Davis, S. L., & Frohman, E. M. (2011). Modeling the mechanisms of Uhthoff's phenomenon in MS patients with internuclear ophthalmoparesis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1233, 313-319.4

Frohman, T. C., Davis, S. L., Beh, S., Greenberg, B. M., Remington, G., & Frohman, E. M. (2013). Uhthoff's phenomena in MS-clinical features and pathophysiology. Nature Reviews Neurology, 9, 535-540.

Frohman, T. C., Frohman, E. M., O'Suilleabhain, P., Salter, A., Dewey, R. B. Jr., Hogan, N., Galetta, S., Lee, A. G., Straumann, D., Noseworthy, J., Zee, D., Corbett, J., Corboy, J., Rivera, V. M., & Kramer, P. D. (2003). Accuracy of clinical detection of INO in MS: Corroboration with quantitative infrared oculography. Neurology, 61, 848-850.

Fuchs, I., Ansorge, U., Redies, C., & Leder, H. (2011). Salience in paintings: Bottom-up influences on eye fixations. Cognitive Computation, 3, 25-36.

Back To Top

G

Gabay, S., Henik, A., & Gradstein, L. (2010). Ocular motor ability and covert attention in patients with duane retraction syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3102-3109.

Gabay, S., Pertzov, Y., & Henik, A. (2011). Orienting of attention, pupil size, and the norepinephrine system. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 123-129.

Gabay, S., Pertzov, Y., Cohen, N., Avidan, G., & Henik, A. (2013). Remapping of the environment without corollary discharges: Evidence from scene-based IOR. Journal of Vision, 13(8):22, 1-10, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/22,doi:10.1167/13.8.22

Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., & Hutzler, F. (2011). Systematic influence of gaze position on pupil size measurement: analysis and correction. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 1171-1181

Gagnier, K. M., Dickinson, C. A., & Intraub, H. (2013). Fixating picture boundaries does not eliminate boundary extension: Implications for scene representation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2161-2186.

Gagnon, D., O'Driscoll, G. A., Petrides, M., & Pike, G. B. (2002). The effect of spatial and temporal information on saccades and neural activity in oculomotor structures. Brain, 125, 123-139.

Gagnon, D., Paus, T., Grosbras, M.-H., Pike, G. B., & O'Driscoll, G. A. (2006). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of frontal oculomotor regions during smooth pursuit. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 458-466.

Gais, S., Köster, S., Sprenger, A., Bethke, J., Heide, W., & Kimmig, H. (2008). Sleep is required for improving reaction times after training on a procedural visuo-motor task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90, 610-615.

Galfano, G., Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2004). Inhibition of return in microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 400-404.

Gallivan, J. P., Chapman, C. S., McLean, D. A., Flanagan, J. R., & Culham, J. C. (2013). Activity patterns in the category-selective occipitotemporal cortex predict upcoming motor actions. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 2408-2424.

Gallivan, J. P., McLean, D. A., Flanagan, J. R., & Culham, J. C. (2013). Where one hand meets the other: limb-specific and action-dependent movement plans decoded from preparatory signals in single human frontoparietal brain areas. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1991-2008.

Galpin, A. J., & Underwood, G. (2005). Eye movements during search and detection in comparative visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1313-1331.

Gamble, A. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2009). The time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 841-847.

Gamble, A. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2010). The time-course of attention to emotional faces in social phobia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 39-44.

Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., Frisson, S., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Processing words and Short Message Service shortcuts in sentential contexts: An eye movement study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 163-179.

Gao, X., Wang, Q., Jackson, T., Zhao, G., Liang, Y., & Chen, H. (2011). Biases in orienting and maintenance of attention among weight dissatisfied women: An eye movement study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 252-259.

Garaas, T. W., & Pomplun, M. (2008). Inspection time and visual-perceptual processing. Vision Research, 48, 523-537.

Garaas, T. W., & Pomplun, M. (2011). Distorted object perception following whole-field adaptation of saccadic eye movements. Journal of Vision, 11(1):2, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/1/2, doi:10.1167/11.1.2.

Garaas, T. W., Nieuwenhuis, T., & Pomplun, M. (2008). A Gaze-contingent paradigm for studying continuous saccadic adaptation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 168, 334-340.

Gautier, J., & Le Meur, O. (2012). A time-dependent saliency model combining center and depth biases for 2D and 3D viewing conditions. Cognitive Computation, 4, 141-156.

Gauvin, H.S., Hartsuiker, R.J., & Huettig, F. (2013). Speech monitoring and phonologically-mediated eye gaze in language perception and production: a comparison using printed word eye-tracking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:818. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00818.

Gaveau, V., Alahyane, N., Salemme, R., & Desmurget, M. (2005). Self-generated saccades do not modify the gain of adapted reactive saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 162, 526-531.

Gaveau, V., Martin, O., Prablanc, C., Pélisson, D., Urquizar, C., & Desmurget, M. (2003). On-line modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals. NeuroReport, 14, 875-878.

Gaveau, V., Pélisson, D., Blangero, A., Urquizar, C., Prablanc, C., Vighetto, A., & Pisella, L. (2008). Saccade control and eye-hand coordination in optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 475-486.

Geng, J. J., & DiQuattro, N. E. (2010). Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid rejection. Journal of Vision, 10(6):5, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/5,doi:10.1167/10.6.5.

Georg, K., & Lappe, M. (2007). Spatio-temporal contingency of saccade-induced chronostasis. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 535-539.

Georg, K., & Lappe, M. (2009). Effects of saccadic adaptation on visual localization before and during saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 192, 9-23.

Georg, K., Hamker, F. H., & Lappe, M. (2008). Influence of adaptation state and stimulus luminance on peri-saccadic localization. Journal of Vision, 8(1):15, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/8/1/15/, doi:10.1167/8.1.15.

Gerardin, P., Gaveau, V., Pélisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (2011). Integration of visual information for saccade production. Human Movement Science, 30, 1009-1021.

Geringswald, F., Herbik, A., Hoffmann, M. B., & Pollmann, S. (2013). Contextual cueing impairment in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Journal of Vision, 13(3):28 1-18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/28, doi:10. 1167/13.3.28.

Gervais, S. J., Holland, A. M., & Dodd, M. D. (2013). My eyes are up here: The nature of the objectifying gaze toward women. Sex Roles, 69, 557-570.

Geyer, T., Müller, H. J., & Krummenacher, J. (2008). Expectancies modulate attentional capture by salient color singletons. Vision Research, 48, 1315-1326.

Geyer, T., Von Mühlenen, A., & Müller, H. J. (2007). What do eye movements reveal about the role of memory in visual search? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7, 924-935.

Ghahghaei, S., Linnell, K. J., Fischer, M. H., Dubey, A., & Davis, R. (2013). Effects of load on the time course of attentional engagement, disengagement, and orienting in reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 453-470.

Gilchrist, I. D., & Harvey, M. (2000). Refixation frequency and memory mechanisms in visual search. Current Biology, 10, 1209-1212.

Gilchrist, I. D., & Harvey, M. (2006). Evidence for a systematic component within scan paths in visual search. Visual Cognition, 14, 704-715.

Gilchrist, I. D., & Proske, H. (2006). Anti-saccades away from faces: evidence for an influence of high-level visual processes on saccade programming. Experimental Brain Research, 173, 708-712.

Gilman, E., & Underwood, G. (2003). Restricting the field of view to investigate the perceptual spans of pianists. Visual Cognition, 10, 201-232.

Gilman, E., & Underwood, G. (2003). The perceptual span during music reading. In J. Hyönä, R. Radach & H. Deubel (Eds.), The Mind's Eyes: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research (pp. 175-191). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Gitchel, G. T., Wetzel, P. A., & Baron, M. S. (2012). Pervasive ocular tremor in patients with parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology, 69, 1011-1017.

Glaholt, M. G., & Reingold, E. M. (2009). Stimulus exposure and gaze bias: A further test of the gaze cascade model. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 445-450.

Glaholt, M. G., & Reingold, E. M. (2009). The time course of gaze bias in visual decision tasks. Visual Cognition, 17, 1228-1243.

Glaholt, M. G., & Reingold, E. M. (2011). Eye movement monitoring as a process tracing methodology in decision making research. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, & Economics, 4, 125-146.

Glaholt, M. G., & Reingold, E. M. (2012). Direct control of fixation times in scene viewing: Evidence from analysis of the distribution of first fixation duration. Visual Cognition, 20, 605-626.

Glaholt, M. G., Rayner, K., & Reingold, E. M. (2012). The mask-onset delay paradigm and the availability of central and peripheral visual information during scene viewing. Journal of Vision, 12(1):9, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/1/9, doi:10.1167/12.1.9.

Glaholt, M. G., Rayner, K., & Reingold, E. M. (2013). Spatial frequency filtering and the direct control of fixation durations during scene viewing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1761-1773.

Glaholt, M. G., Wu, M.-C., & Reingold, E. M. (2009). Predicting preference from fixations. PsychNology Journal, 7, 141-158.

Glaholt, M. G., Wu, M.-C., & Reingold, E. M. (2010). Evidence for top-down control of eye movements during visual decision making. Journal of Vision, 10(5):15, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/content/10/5/15, doi:10.1167/10.5.15.

Glasser, D. M., Tsui, J. M. G., Pack, C. C., & Tadin, D. (2011). Perceptual and neural consequences of rapid motion adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108,E1080-E1088.

Glen, F. C., Smith, N. D., & Crabb, D. P. (2013). Saccadic eye movements and face recognition performance in patients with central glaucomatous visual field defects. Vision Research, 82, 42-51.

Godfroid, A., & Uggen, M. S. (2013). Attention to irregular verbs by beginning learner of German. Studies in Second Language Acquisitio, 35, 291-322.

Godfroid, A., Boers, F., & Housen, A. (2013). An eye for words. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 483-517.

Godijn, R., & Kramer, A. F. (2006). Prosaccades and antisaccades to onsets and color singletons: Evidence that erroneous prosaccades are not reflexive. Experimental Brain Research, 172, 439-448.

Godijn, R., & Kramer, A. F. (2007). Antisaccade costs with static and dynamic targets. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 802-815.

Godijn, R., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Oculomotor capture by surprising onsets. Visual Cognition, 16, 279-289.

Godijn, R., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). The effect of attentional demands on the antisaccade cost. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 795-806.

Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Oculomotor capture and inhibition of return. Psychological Research, 66, 234-246.

Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Programming of exogenous and endogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1039-1054.

Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2003). Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 882-896.

Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2004). The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 538-554.

Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Overt is no better than covert when rehearsing visuo-spatial information in working memory. Memory & Cognition, 40, 52-61.

Godwin, H. J., Benson, V., & Drieghe, D. (2013). Using interrupted visual displays to explore the capacity, time course, and format of fixation plans during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39,1700-1712,

Godwin, H. J., Hyde, S., Taunton, D., Calver, J., Blake, J. I. R., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). The influence of expertise on maritime driving behaviour. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 483-492.

Golding, C. V. P., Danchaivijitr, C., Hodgson, T. L., Tabrizi, S. J., & Kennard, C. (2006). Identification of an oculomotor biomarker of preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology, 67, 485-487.

Gollan, T. H., Slattery, T. J., Goldenberg, D., van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Rayner, K. (2011). Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: The frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 186-209.

Gomi, H., Abekawa, N., & Shimojo, S. (2013). The hand sees visual periphery better than the eye: Motor-dependent visual motion analyses. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 16502-16509.

Gonzalez, C. C., & Burke, M. R. (2013). The brain uses efference copy information to optimise spatial memory. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 189-197.

Gonzalez, E. G., Wong, A. M., Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Tarita-Nistor, L., & Steinbach, M. J. (2012). Eye position stability in amblyopia and in normal binocular vision. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 53, 5386-5394.

González, E. G., Lillakas, L., Lam, A., Gallie, B.L., & Steinbach, M. J. (2013). Horizontal saccade dynamics after childhood monocular enucleation. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54, 6463-6471.

Gooding, D. C. (1999). Antisaccade task performance in questionnaire-identified schizotypes. Schizophrenia Research, 35, 157-166.

Gooding, D. C., & Tallent, K. A. (2001). The association between antisaccade task and working memory task performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 8-16.

Gooding, D. C., Grabowski, J. A., & Hendershot, C. S. (2000). Fixation stability in schizophrenia, bipolar, and control subjects. Psychiatry Research, 97, 119-128.

Gooding, D. C., Miller, M. D., & Kwapil, T. R. (2000). Smooth pursuit eye tracking and visual fixation in psychosis-prone individuals. Psychiatry Research, 93, 41-54.

Gooding, D. C., Mohapatra, L., & Shea, H. B. (2004). Temporal stability of saccadic task performance in schizophrenia and bipolar patients. Psychological Medicine, 34, 921-932.

Gooding, D. C., Shea, H. B., & Matts, C. W. (2005). Saccadic performance in questionnaire-identifiedschizotypes over time. Psychiatry Research, 133, 173-186.

Gorbet, D. J., & Sergio, L. E. (2009). The behavioral consequences of dissociating the spatial directions of eye and arm movements. Brain Research, 1284, 77-88.

Gordon, P. C., & Moser, S. (2007). Insight into analogies: Evidence from eye movements. Visual Cognition, 15, 20-35.

Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Johnson, M., & Lee, Y. (2006). Similarity-based interference during language comprehension: Evidence from eye tracking during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 1304-1321.

Gordon, P. C., Plummer, P., & Choi, W. (2013). See before you jump: Full recognition of parafoveal words precedes skips during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 633-641.

Gordon, R. D., & Vollmer, S. D. (2010). Episodic representation of diagnostic and nondiagnostic object colour. Visual Cognition, 18, 728-750.

Gordon, R. D., Vollmer, S. D., & Frankl, M. L. (2008). Object continuity and the transsaccadic representation of form. Perception and Psychophysics, 70, 667-679.

Gorgoraptis, N., Catalao, R. F. G., Bays, P. M., & Husain, M. (2011). Dynamic updating of working memory resources for visual objects. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8502-8511.

Goujon, A., Brockmole, J. R., & Ehinger, K. A. (2012). How visual and semantic information influence learning in familiar contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1315-1327.

Gozli, D. G., Chow, A., Chasteen, A. L., & Pratt, J. (2013). Valence and vertical space: Saccade trajectory deviations reveal metaphorical spatial activation. Visual Cognition, 21, 628-646.

Graham, D. J., & Jeffery, R. W. (2011). Location, location, location: Eye-tracking evidence that consumers preferentially view prominently positioned nutrition information. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 111, 1704-1711.

Graham, D. J., & Jeffery, R. W. (2012). Predictors of nutrition label viewing during food purchase decision making: an eye tracking investigation. Public Health Nutrition, 15, 189-197.

Granek, J. A., Pisella, L., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., & Sergio, L. E. (2012). The role of the caudal superior parietal lobule in updating hand location in peripheral vision: Further evidence from Optic Ataxia. PLoS ONE 7(10): e46619. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046619.

Grant, M., Clifton, C. Jr., & Frazier, L.(2012). The role of Non-Actuality Implicatures in processing elided constituents. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 326-343.

Granzier, J. J., Toscani, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Role of eye movements in chromatic induction. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 29, A353-65. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A353.

Graupner, S.-T., Pannasch, S., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2011). Saccadic context indicates information processing within visual fixations: Evidence from event-related potentials and eye-movements analysis of the distractor effect. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80, 54-62.

Graupner, S.-T., Velichkovsky, B. M., Pannasch, S., & Marx, J. (2007). Surprise, surprise: Two distinct components in the visually evoked distractor effect. Psychophysiology, 44, 251-261.

Green, M. J., Waldron, J. H., Simpson, I., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Visual processing of social context during mental state perception in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 33, 34-42.

Greenberg, S. N., Inhoff, A. W., & Weger, U. W. (2006). The impact of letter detection on eye movement patterns during reading: Reconsidering lexical analysis in connected text as a function of task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 987-995.

Greenberg, T., Carlson, J. M., Cha, J, Hajcak, G., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2013). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex reactivity is altered in generalized anxiety disorder during fear generalization. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 242-250.

Greenberg, T., Carlson, J. M., Cha, J., Hajcak, G., & Mujica-Parod, L. R. (2013). Neural reactivity tracks fear generalization gradients. Biological Psychology, 92, 2-8.

Greene, H. H. (2006). The control of fixation duration in visual search. Perception, 35, 303-315.

Greene, H. H. (2008). Distance-from-target dynamics during visual search. Vision Research, 48, 2476-2484.

Greene, H. H., & Rayner, K. (2001). Eye movement control in direction-coded visual search. Perception, 30, 147-157.

Greene, H. H., & Rayner, K. (2001). Eye movements and familiarity effects in visual search. Vision Research, 41, 3763-3773.

Greene, H. H., Brown, J. M., & Paradis, B. A. (2013). Luminance contrast and the visual span during visual target localization. Displays, 34, 27-32.

Greene, H. H., Pollatsek, A., Masserang, K., Lee, Y. J., & Rayner, K. (2010). Directional processing within the perceptual span during visual target localization. Vision Research, 50, 1274-1282.

Greene, H. H., Simpson, D., & Bennion, J. (2012). The perceptual span during foveally-demanding visual target localization. Acta Psychologica, 139, 434-439.

Greene, M. R., Liu, T., & Wolfe, J. M. (2012). Reconsidering Yarbus: A failure to predict observers' task from eye movement patterns. Vision Research, 62, 1-8.

Greenwald, H. S., & Knill, D. C. (2009). Cue integration outside central fixation: A study of grasping in depth. Journal of Vision, 9(2):11, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/9/2/11/, doi:10.1167/9.2.11.

Gregory, N. J., & Hodgson, T. L. (2012). Giving subjects the eye and showing them the finger: Socio-biological cues and saccade generation in the anti-saccade task. Perception, 41, 131-147.

Greschner, M., Bongard, M., Rujan, P., & Ammermüll, J. (2002). Retinal ganglion cell synchronization by fixational eye movements improves feature estimation. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 341-347.

Grewal, P., Viswanathan, J., Barton, J. J. S., & Lanyon, L. J. (2012). Line bisection under an attentional gradient induced by simulated neglect in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1190-1201.

Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274-279.

Griffiths, G., Herwig, A., & Schneider, W. X. (2013). Stimulus localization interferes with stimulus recognition: Evidence from an attentional blink paradigm. Journal of Vision, 13(7):7, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/7/7, doi:10.1167/13.7.7.

Grill-Spector, K., Kushnir, T., Hendler, T., & Malach, R. (2000). The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognition performance in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 837-843.

Grodner, D. J., Klein, N. M., Carbary, K. M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2010). "Some",and possibly all, scalar inferences are not delayed: Evidence for immediate pragmatic enrichment. Cognition, 116, 42-55.

Groen, M., & Noyes, J. (2010). Solving problems: How can guidance concerning task-relevancy be provided? Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 1318-1326.

Groen, M., & Noyes, j. (2013). Establishing goals and maintaining coherence in multiparty computer-mediated communication. Discourse Processes, 50, 85-106.

Grondelaers, S., Speelman, D., Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., & Geeraerts, D. (2009). Introducing a new entity into discourse: Comprehension and production evidence for the status of Dutch er "there" as a higher-level expectancy monitor. Acta Psychologica, 130, 153-160.

Grosjean, M., Rinkenauer, G., & Jainta, S. (2012). Where do the eyes really go in the hollow-face i.llusion? PLoS ONE 7(9): e44706. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044706.

Grubb, M. A., Behrmann, M., Egan, R., Minshew, N. J., Heeger, D. J., & Carrasco, M. (2013). Exogenous spatial attention: Evidence for intact functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Vision, 13(14):9, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/14/9, doi:10.1167/13.14.9.

Guan, S., Liu, Y., Xia, R., & Zhang, M. (2012). Covert attention regulates saccadic reaction time by routing between different visual-oculomotor pathways. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 1748 - 1755.

Guerin, S. A., Robbins, C. A., Gilmore, A. W., & Schacter, D. L. (2012). Retrieval failure contributes to gist-based false recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 68-78.

Guerreiro, M. J. S., & Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2011). Now you see it, now you don't: Evidence for age-dependent and age-independent cross-modal distraction. Psychology and Aging, 26, 415-426.

Guerreiro, M. J. S., Adam, J. J., & Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2012). Automatic selective attention as a function of sensory modality in aging. The Journal of Gerontology, 67B: 194 - 202.

Guerreiro, M. J. S., Murphy, D. R., & Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2013). Making sense of age-related distractibility: The critical role of sensory modality. Acta Psychologica, 142, 184-194.

Guez, J., Morris, A. P., & Krekelberg, B. (2013). Intrasaccadic suppression is dominated by reduced detector gain. Journal of Vision, 13(8):4, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/4, doi:10.1167/13.8.4.

Guillaume, A. (2012). Saccadic inhibition is accompanied by large and complex amplitude modulations when induced by visual backward masking. Journal of Vision, 12(6):5, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/6/5, doi:10.1167/12.6.5.

Guitart-Masip, M., Barnes, G. R., Horner, A., Bauer, M., Dolan, R. J., & Duzel, E. (2013).Synchronization of medial temporal lobe and prefrontal rhythms in human decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 442-451.

Guo, F., Preston, T. J., Das, K., Giesbrecht, B., & Eckstein, M. P. (2012). Feature-independent neural coding of target detection during search of natural scenes. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 9499-9510.

Gupta, R., & Raymond, J. E. (2012). Emotional distraction unbalances visual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 184-189.

Gutteling, T. P., van Ettinger-Veenstra, H. M., Kenemans, J. L., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2010). Lateralized frontal eye field activity precedes occipital activity shortly before saccades: Evidence for cortico-cortical feedback as a mechanism underlying covert attention shifts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1931-1943.

Guyader, N., Malsert, J., & Marendaz, C. (2010). Having to identify a target reduces latencies in prosaccades but not in antisaccades. Psychological Research, 74, 12-20.

Guzman-Martinez, E., Leung, P., Franconeri, S., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2009). Rapid eye-fixation training without eyetracking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 491-496.

Guérard, K., Saint-Aubin, J., Boucher, P., & Tremblay, S. (2011). The role of awareness in anticipation and recall performance in the Hebb repetition paradigm: implications for sequence learning. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1012-1022.

Guérard, K., Saint-Aubin, J., Poirier, M., & Demetriou, C. (2012). Assessing the influence of letter position in reading normal and transposed texts using a letter detection task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 227-238.

Guérard, K.,Saint-Aubin, J., & Maltais, M. (2013). The role of verbal memory in regressions during reading. Memory & Cognition, 41, 122-136.

Gysen, V., De Graef, P., & Verfaillie, K. (2002). Detection of intrasaccadic displacements and depth rotations of moving objects. Vision Research, 42, 379-391.

Gysen, V., Verfaillie, K., & De Graef, P. (2002). The effect of stimulus blanking on the detection of intrasaccadic displacements of translating objects. Vision Research, 42, 2021-2030.

Gysen, V., Verfaillie, K., & De Graef, P. (2002). Transsaccadic perception of translating objects: Effects of landmark objects and visual field position. Vision Research, 42, 1785-1796.

Görges, F., Oppermann, F., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H. (2013). Activation of phonological competitors in visual search. Acta Psychologica, 143, 168-175.

Back To Top

H

Habekost, T., & Starrfelt, R. (2006). Alexia and quadrant-amblyopia: Reading disability after a minor visual field deficit. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2465-2476.

Hafed, Z. M. (2013). Alteration of visual perception prior to microsaccades. Neuron, 77, 775-786.

Hagemann, N., Schorer, J., Cañal-Bruland, R., Lotz, S., Strauss, B. (2010). Visual perception in fencing: Do the eye movements of fencers represent their information pickup? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2204-2214.

Hagler, D. Jr., Riecke, L., & Sereno, M. I. (2007). Parietal and superior frontal visuospatial maps activated by pointing and saccades. NeuroImage, 35, 1562-1577.

Hahn, B., Robinson, B. M., Harvey, A. N., Kaiser, S. T., Leonard, C. J., Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. M. (2012). Visuospatial attention in schizophrenia: Deficits in broad monitoring. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 119-128.

Haith, A. M., Reppert, T. R., & Shadmehr, R. (2012). Evidence for hyperbolic temporal discounting of reward in control of movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 11727-11736.

Hall, J. K., Hutton, S. B., & Morgan, M. J. (2010). Sex differences in scanning faces: Does attention to the eyes explain female superiority in facial expression recognition? Cognition & Emotion, 24, 629-637.

Hamborg, K.-C., Bruns, M., Ollermann, F., & Kaspar, K. (2012). The effect of banner animation on fixation behavior and recall performance in search tasks. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 576-282.

Hamid, S. N., Stankiewicz, B., & Hayhoe, M. (2010). Gaze patterns in navigation: Encoding information in large-scale environments. Journal of Vision, 10(12):28, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/12/28, doi:10.1167/10.12.28.

Hamilton, D. A., Johnson, T. E., Redhead, E. S., & Verney, S. P. (2009). Control of rodent and human spatial navigation by room and apparatus cues. Behavioural Processes, 81, 154-169.

Hamker, F. H., Zirnsak, M., & Lappe, M. (2008). About the influence of post-saccadic mechanisms for visual stability on peri-saccadic compression of object location. Journal of Vision, 8(14):1, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/1/, doi:10.1167/8.14.1.

Hampson, R. E., Opris, I., & Deadwyler, S. A. (2010). Neural correlates of fast pupil dilation in nonhuman primates: Relation to behavioral performance and cognitive workload. Behavioural Brain Research, 212, 1-11.

Han, P., Saunders, D. R., Woods, R. L., & Luo, G. (2013). Trajectory prediction of saccadic eye movements using a compressed exponential model. 13(8):27, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/27, doi:10.1167/13.8.27.

Hancock, D. J., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2013). Gaze behaviors and decision making accuracy of higher- and lower-level ice hockey referees. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14, 66-71.

Hand, C. J., O’Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2012). Word-initial letters influence fixation durations during fluent reading. Frontiers in Psychology, 3: 85. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00085.

Hanisch, C., Radach, R., Holtkamp, K., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., & Konrad, K. (2006). Oculomotor inhibition in children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Neural Transmission, 113, 671-684.

Hanks, T. D., Mazurek, M. E., Kiani, R., Hopp, E., & Shadlen, M. N. (2011). Elapsed decision time affects the weighting of prior probability in a perceptual decision task. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 6339-6352.

Hannus, A., Cornelissen, F. W., Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2005). Selection-for-action in visual search. Acta Psychologica, 118, 171-191.

Hannus, A., van den Berg, R., Bekkering, H., Roerdink, J. B. T. M., & Cornelissen, F. W. (2006). Visual search near threshold: Some features are more equal than others. Journal of Vision, 6, 523-540.

Hansen, B. J., Chelaru, M. I., & Dragoi, V. (2012). Correlated variability in laminar cortical circuits. Neuron, 76, 590-602.

Hanulíková, A., & Weber, A. (2012). Sink positive: Linguistic experience with th substitutions influences nonnative word recognition. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 613-629.

Harkin, B., Miellet, S., & Kessler, K. (2012). What checkers actually check: An eye tracking study of inhibitory control and working memory. PLoS ONE 7(9): e44689. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044689.

Harris, A. M., Remington, R. W., & Becker, S. I. (2013). Feature specificity in attentional capture by size and color. Journal of Vision, 13(3):12, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/12, doi:10.1167/13.3.12.

Harris, J. A., Clifton, C., Jr & Frazier, L. (2013). Processing and domain selection: Quantificational variability effects. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1519-1544.

Harris, J., Pylkkänen, L., McElree, B., & Frisson, S. (2008). The cost of question concealment: Eye-tracking and MEG evidence. Brain and Language, 107, 44-61.

Harris, L. R., & Smith, A. T. (2000). Interactions between first- and second-order motion revealed by optokinetic nystagmus. Experimental Brain Research, 130, 67-72.

Harrison, W. J., Mattingley, J. B., & Remington, R. W. (2012). Pre-saccadic shifts of visual attention. PLoS ONE 7(9): e45670. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045670.

Harrison, W. J., Mattingley, J. B., Remington, R. W. (2013). Eye movement targets are released from visual crowding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 2927-2933.

Harry, B., Davis, C., & Kim, J. (2012). Exposure in central vision facilitates view-invariant face recognition in the periphery. Journal of Vision, 12(2):13, 1-9, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/2/13, doi:10.1167/12.2.13.

Hart, B. M., Vockeroth, J., Schumann, F., Bartl, K., Schneider, E., König, P., & Einhäuser, W. (2009). Gaze allocation in natural stimuli: Comparing free exploration to head-fixed viewing conditions. Visual Cognition, 17, 1132-1158.

Hartmann, T. S., Bremmer, F., Albright, T. D., & Krekelberg, B. (2011). Receptive field positions in area MT during slow eye movements. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 10437-10444.

Hartwig, A., Charman, W. N., & Radhakrishnan, H. (2011). Accommodative response to peripheral stimuli in myopes and emmetropes. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 31, 91-99.

Hartwig, A., Gowen, E., Charman, W. N., & Radhakrishnan, H. (2011). Analysis of head position used by myopes and emmetropes when performing a near-vision reading task. Vision Research, 51, 1712-1717.

Hartwig, A., Gowen, E., Charman, W. N., & Radhakrishnan, H. (2011). Working distance and eye and head movements during near work in myopes and non-myopes. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 94, 536-544.

Hartwig, J., Schnitzspahn, K. M., Kliegel, M., Velichkovsky, B. M., & Helmert, J. R. (2013). I see you remembering: What eye movements can reveal about process characteristics of prospective memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88, 193-199.

Harvey, A. J., Kneller, W., & Campbell, A. C. (2013). The effects of alcohol intoxication on attention and memory for visual scenes. Memory, 21, 969-980.

Harvey, A. J., Kneller, W., & Campbell, A. C. (2013). The elusive effects of alcohol intoxication on visual attention and eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 617-624.

Harvey, B. M., & Dumoulin, S. O. (2011). The relationship between cortical magnification factor and population receptive field size in human visual cortex: Constancies in cortical architecture. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 13604-13612;

Harvey, B. M., Braddick, O. J., & Cowey, A. (2010). Similar effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of MT+ and a dorsomedial extrastriate site including V3A on pattern detection and position discrimination of rotating and radial motion patterns. Journal of Vision, 10(5):21, 1–15, http://journalofvision.org/content/10/5/21, doi:10.1167/10.5.21.

Harvey, M., Gilchrist, I. D., Olk, B., & Muir, K. (2003). Eye-movement patterns do not mediate size distortion effects in hemispatial neglect: Looking without seeing. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1114-1121.

Harvey, M., Olk, B., Muir, K., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Manual responses and saccades in chronic and recovered hemispatial neglect: A study using visual search. Neuropsychologia, 40, 705-717.

Hassler, U., Friese, U., Martens, U., Trujillo-Barreto, N., & Gruber, T. (2013). Repetition priming effects dissociate between miniature eye movements and induced gamma-band responses in the human electroencephalogram. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 2425-2433,

Hasson, U., Hendler, T., Bashat, D. B., & Malach, R. (2001). Vase or face? A neural correlate of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 744-753.

Havermann, K., & Lappe, M. (2010). The influence of the consistency of post-saccadic visual error on saccadic adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 3302-3310.

Havermann, K., Volcic, R., & Lappe, M. (2012). Saccadic adaptation to moving targets. PLoS ONE 7(6): e39708. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039708.

Havermann, K., Zimmermann, E., & Lappe, M. (2011). Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 2536-2545.

Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2005). Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research, 45, 855-863.

Hawelka, S., Gagl, B., & Wimmer, H. (2010). A dual-route perspective on eye movements of dyslexic readers. Cognition, 115, 367-379.

Hawelka, S., Schuster, S., Gagl, B., & Hutzler, F. (2013). Beyond single syllables: The effect of first syllable frequency and orthographic similarity on eye movements during silent reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1134-1153.

Hawes, R. (2009). Vision and reality: relativity in art. Digital Creativity, 20, 177-186.

Hayashi, R., & Tanifuji, M. (2012). Which image is in awareness during binocular rivalry? Reading perceptual status from eye movements. Journal of Vision, 12(3):5, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/3/5, doi:10.1167/12.3.5.

Hayashi, R., Sugita, Y., Nishida, S., & Kawano, K. (2010). How motion signals are integrated across frequencies: Study on motion perception and ocular following responses using multiple-slit stimuli. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 230-243.

Hayden, B. Y., & Gallant, J. L. (2005). Time course of attention reveals different mechanisms for spatial and feature-based attention in area V4. Neuron, 47, 637-643.

Hayden, B. Y., & Gallant, J. L. (2009). Combined effects of spatial and feature-based attention on responses of V4 neurons. Vision Research, 49, 1182-1187.

Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Temporal discounting predicts risk sensitivity in rhesus macaques. Current Biology, 17, 49-53.

Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2010). Neurons in anterior cingulate cortex multiplex information about reward and action. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3339-3346.

Hayden, B. Y., Heilbronner, S. R., & Platt, M. L. (2010). Ambiguity aversion in rhesus macaques. Frontier in Neuroscience, 4:166. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00166.

Hayden, B. Y., Heilbronner, S. R., Nair, A. C., & Platt, M. L. (2008). Cognitive influences on risk-seeking by rhesus macaques. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 389-395.

Hayden, B. Y., Heilbronner, S. R., Pearson, J. M., & Platt, M. L. (2011). Surprise signals in anterior cingulate cortex: Neuronal encoding of unsigned reward prediction errors driving adjustment in behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4178 - 4187.

Hayden, B. Y., Nair, A. C., McCoy, A. N., & Platt, M. L. (2008). Posterior cingulate cortex mediates outcome-contingent allocation of behavior. Neuron, 60, 19-25.

Hayden, B. Y., Pearson, J. M., & Platt, M. L. (2011). Neuronal basis of sequential foraging decisions in a patchy environment. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 933-939.

Hayden, B. Y., Smith, D. V., & Platt, M. L. (2009). Electrophysiological correlates of default-mode processing in macaque posterior cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 5948-5953.

Hayden, B. Y., Smith, D. V., & Platt, M. L. (2010). Cognitive control signals in posterior cingulate cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4:223. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00223.

Hayden, B.Y., & Gallant, J.L. (2013). Working memory and decision processes in visual area V4. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7:18. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00018.

Hayes, T. R., Petrov, A. A., & Sederberg, P. B. (2011). A novel method for analyzing sequential eye movements reveals strategic influence on Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Journal of Vision, 11(10):10, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/10/10, doi:10.1167/11.10.10.

He, J., & McCarley, J. S. (2010). Executive working memory load does not compromise perceptual processing during visual search: Evidence from additive factors analysis. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 308-316.

He, W., Brock, J., Johnson, B. W. (2014). Face-sensitive brain responses measured from a four year old child with a custom-sized child MEG system. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 213-217.

He, X., Wang, L. & Chen, Y. (2013). The experimental research of task load quantitative analysis based on the pupil diameter. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8019, 125-133

Heaver, B., & Hutton, S. B. (2011). Keeping an eye on the truth? Pupil size changes associated with recognition memory. Memory, 19, 398-405.

Hedge, C., & Leonards, U. (2013). Using eye movements to explore switch costs in working memory. Journal of Vision, 13(4):18, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/4/18, doi:10.1167/13.4.18.

Hegarty, M., Canham, M. S., & Fabrikan, S. I. (2010). Thinking about the weather: How display salience and knowledge affect performance in a graphic inference task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 37-53.

Hegarty, M., Smallman, H. S., & Stull, A. T. (2012). Choosing and using geospatial displays: Effects of design on performance and metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 1-17.

Heilbronner, S. R., & Platt, M. L. (2013). Causal evidence of performance monitoring by neurons in posterior cingulate cortex during learning. Neuron, 80, 1384-1391.

Heilbronner, S. R., Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2011). Decision salience signals in posterior cingulate cortex. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00055.

Heine, A., Thaler, V., Tamm, S., Hawelka, S., Schneider, M., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., Stern, E., & Jacobs, A. M. (2010). What the eyes already 'know': using eye movement measurement to tap into children's implicit numerical magnitude representations. Infant and Child Development, 19, 175 - 186.

Heinen, S. J., Jin, Z., & Watamaniuk, S. N. J. (2011). Flexibility of foveal attention during ocular pursuit. Journal of Vision, 11(2):9, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/2/9, doi:10.1167/11.2.9.

Heisz, J. J., & Ryan, J. D. (2011). The effects of prior exposure on face processing in younger and older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 3:15. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00015.

Heisz, J. J., & Shore, D. I. (2008). More efficient scanning for familiar faces. Journal of Vision, 8(1):9, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/8/1/9/, doi:10.1167/8.1.9.

Heisz, J. J., Pottruff, M. M., & Shore, D. I. (2013). Females scan more than males: A potential mechanism for sex differences in recognition memory. Psychological Science, 24, 1157-1163.

Heller, D., & Chambers, C. G. (2014). Would a blue kite by any other name be just as blue? Effects of descriptive choices on subsequent referential behavior. Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 53-67.

Helmchen, C., Glasauer, S., & Sprenger, A. (2013). Inverse eye position dependency of downbeat nystagmus in midline medullary lesion. Journal of Neurology, 260, 2908-2910.

Helmchen, C., Gottschalk, S., Sander, T., Trillenberg, P., Rambold, H., & Sprenger, A. (2007). Beneficial effects of 3,4-diaminopyridine on positioning downbeat nystagmus in a circumscribed uvulo-nodular lesion. Journal of Neurology, 254, 1126-1128.

Helmchen, C., Pohlmann, J., Trillenberg, P., Lencer, R., Graf, J., & Sprenger, A. (2012). Role of anticipation and prediction in smooth pursuit eye movement control in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorder, 27, 1012-1018.

Helmchen, C., Sprenger, A., Rambold, H., Sander, T., Kompf, D., & Straumann, D. (2004). Effect of 3,4-diaminopyridine on the gravity dependence of ocular drift in downbeat nystagmus. Neurology, 63, 752-753.

Helmert, J. R., Pannasch, S., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2008). Influences of dwell time and cursor control on the performance in gaze driven typing. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(4):3, 1-8.

Henderson, J. M., & Luke, S. G. (2012). Oculomotor inhibition of return in normal and mindless reading. Psychonomic Bulltin & Review, 19, 1101-1107.

Henderson, J. M., & Pierce, G. L. (2008). Eye movements during scene viewing: Evidence for mixed control of fixation durations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 566-573.

Henderson, J. M., & Smith, T. J. (2009). How are eye fixation durations controlled during scene viewing? Further evidence from a scene onset delay paradigm. Visual Cognition, 17, 1055-1082.

Henderson, J. M., Chanceaux, M., & Smith, T. J. (2009). The influence of clutter on real-world scene search: Evidence from search efficiency and eye movements. Journal of Vision, 9(1):32, 1-8, http://journalofvision.org/9/1/32/, doi:10.1167/9.1.32.

Henderson, J. M., Luke, S. G., Schmidt, J., & Richards, J. E. (2013). Co-registration of eye movements and event-related potentials in connected-text paragraph reading. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7(28), 1-13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00028.

Henderson, J. M., Malcolm, G. L., & Schandl, C. (2009). Searching in the dark: Cognitive relevance drives attention in real-world scenes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 850-856.

Henderson, J. M., Nuthmann, A., & Luke, S. G. (2013). Eye movement control during scene viewing: Immediate effects of scene luminance on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 318-322.

Henderson, J.M., Shinkareva, S.V., Wang, J., Luke, S.G., & Olejarczyk, J. (2013). Predicting cognitive state from eye movements. PLoS ONE 8(5): e64937. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064937.

Herman, J.P., Cloud, C.P., & Wallman, J. (2013). End-point variability is not noise in saccade adaptation. PLoS ONE 8(3): e59731. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059731.

Hermens, F., & Walker, R. (2010). Gaze and arrow distractors influence saccade trajectories similarly. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2120-2140.

Hermens, F., & Walker, R. (2010). The influence of onsets and offsets on saccade programming. i-Perception, 1, 83-94.

Hermens, F., & Walker, R. (2010). What determines the direction of microsaccades? Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(4):1, 1-20.

Hermens, F., & Walker, R. (2012). Do you look where I look? Attention shifts and response preparation following dynamic social cues. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(5):5, 1-11.

Hermens, F., & Walker, R. (2012). The site of interference in the saccadic Stroop effect. Vision Research, 73, 10-22.

Hermens, F., & Zanker, J. (2012). Looking at Op Art: Gaze stability and motion illusions. i-Perception, 3, 282-304.

Hermens, F., Ghose, T., & Wagemans, J. (2013). Advance information modulates the global effect even without instruction on where to look. Experimental Brain Research, 226, 639-648.

Hermens, F., Sumner, P., & Walker, R. (2010). Inhibition of masked primes as revealed by saccade curvature. Vision Research, 50, 46-56.

Hermens, F., Zanker, J. M., & Walker, R. (2010). Microsaccades and preparatory set: A comparison between delayed and immediate, exogenous and endogenous pro- and anti-saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 489-498.

Hernandez, T. D., Levitan, C. A., Banks, M. S., & Schor, C. M. (2008). How does saccade adaptation affect visual perception? Journal of Vision, 8(8):3, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/8/8/3/, doi:10.1167/8.8.3.

Herrington, T. M., Masse, N. Y., Hachmeh, K. J., Smith, J. E. T., Assad, J. A., & Cook, E. P. (2009). The effect of microsaccades on the correlation between neural activity and behavior in middle temporal, ventral intraparietal, and lateral intraparietal areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 5793-5805.

Herrmann, K., Heeger, D. J., & Carrasco, M. (2012). Feature-based attention enhances performance by increasing response gain. Vision Research, 74, 10-20.

Herrmann, K., Montaser-Kouhsari, L., Carrasco, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). When size matters: attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 1554-1559.

Hertel, S., Sprenger, A., Klein, C., Kömpf, D., Helmchen, C., & Kimmig, H. (2009). Different saccadic abnormalities in PINK1 mutation carriers and in patients with non-genetic. Journal of Neurology, 25, 1192-1194.

Hertle, R. W., Felius, J., Yang, D., & Kaufman, M. (2009). Eye muscle surgery for infantile nystagmus syndrome in the first two years of life. Clinical Ophthalmology, 3, 615-624.

Hertle, R. W., Yang, D., Adams, K., & Caterino, R. (2011). Surgery for the treatment of vertical head posturing associated with infantile nystagmus syndrome: results in 24 patients. Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology, 39, 37-46.

Herwig, A, & Horstmann, G. (2011). Action-effect associations revealed by eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 531-537.

Herwig, A., Beisert, M., & Schneider, W. X. (2010). On the spatial interaction of visual working memory and attention: Evidence for a global effect from memory-guided saccades. Journal of Vision, 10(5):8, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/content/10/5/8, doi:10.1167/10.5.8.

Hesse, C., Ball, K., & Schenk, T. (2012). Visuomotor performance based on peripheral vision is impaired in the visual form agnostic patient DF. Neuropsychologia, 50, 90-97.

Hesse, C., Nakagawa, T. T., & Deubel, H. (2010). Bimanual movement control is moderated by fixation strategies. Experimental Brain Research, 202, 837-850.

Hicheur, H., Zozor, S., Campagne, A., & Chauvin, A. (2013). Microsaccades are modulated by both attentional demands of a visual discrimination task and background noise. Journal of Vision, 13(13):18, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/13/18, doi:10.1167/13.13.18.

Hickey, C., & van Zoest, W. (2013). Reward-associated stimuli capture the eyes in spite of strategic attentional set. Vision Research, 92, 67-74.

Hickman, S. J., Raoof, N., McLean, R. J., & Gottlob, I. (2014). Vision and multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis and Related Disorders, 3, 3-16.

Hicks, S.L., Sharma, R., Khan, A.N., Berna, C.M., Waldecker, A., Talbot, K., Kennard, C., & Turner, M. R. (2013). An eye-tracking version of the Trail-Making Test. PLoS ONE 8(12): e84061. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0084061.

Higenell, V., White, B. J., Hwang, J. R., & Munoz, D. P. (2013). Localizing the neural substrate of reflexive covert orienting. Journal of eye Movement Research, 6(1):1, 1-14.

Higgins, J. S., & Wang, R. F. (2010). A landmark effect in the perceived displacement of objects. Vision Research, 50, 242-248.

Higgins, J. S., Irwin, D. E., Wang, R. F., & Thomas, L. E. (2009). Visual direction constancy across eyeblinks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1607-1617.

Hilchey, M. D., Klein, R. M., & Ivanoff, J. (2012). Perceptual and motor inhibition of return: components or flavors? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1416-1429.

Hilchey, M. D., Klein, R. M., Satel, J., & Wang, Z. (2012). Oculomotor inhibition of return: How soon is it "recoded" into spatiotopic coordinates? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1145-1153.

Hilchey, M. D., Satel, J., Ivanoff, J., & Klein, R. M. (2013). On the nature of the delayed "inhibitory". Cueing effects generated by uninformative arrows at fixation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 593-600.

Hillstrom, A. P., Scholey, H., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2012). The effect of the first glimpse at a scene on eye movements during search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 204-210.

Hine, T. J., Wallis, G., Wood, J. M., & Stavrou, E. P. (2006). Reflexive optokinetic nystagmus in younger and older observers under photopic and mesopic viewing conditions. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 47, 5288-5294.

Hirai, M., Saunders, D. R., & Troje, N. F. (2011). Allocation of attention to biological motion: Local motion dominates global shape. Journal of Vision, 11(3):4, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/3/4, doi:10.1167/11.3.4.

Hirose, Y., Kennedy, A., & Tatler, B. W. (2010). Perception and memory across viewpoint changes in moving images. Journal of Vision, 10(4):2, 1-19, http://journalofvision.org/10/4/2/, doi:10.1167/10.4.2.

Hisakata, R., & Murakami, I. (2008). The effects of eccentricity and retinal illuminance on the illusory motion seen in a stationary luminance gradient. Vision Research, 48, 1940-1948.

Hisakata, R., Terao, M., & Murakami, I. (2013). Illusory position shift induced by motion within a moving envelope during smooth-pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision, 13(12):21, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/12/21, doi:10.1167/13.12.21.

Ho-Phuoc, T., Guyader, N., & Guérin-Dugué, A. (2010). A functional and statistical bottom-up saliency model to reveal the relative contributions of low-level visual guiding factors. Cognitive Computation, 2, 344-359.

Ho-Phuoc, T., Guyader, N., Landragin, F., & Guérin-Dugué, A. (2012). When viewing natural scenes, do abnormal colors impact on spatial or temporal parameters of eye movements? Journal of Vision, 12(2):4, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision. org/content/12/2/4, doi:10.1167/12.2.4.

Hobson, J., Bruce, G., & Butler, S. H. (2013). A flicker change blindness task employing eye tracking reveals an association with levels of craving not consumption. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 27, 93-97.

Hochpöchler, U., Schnotz, W., Rasch, T., Ullrich, M., Horz, H., McElvany, N., & Baumert, J. (2013). Dynamics of mental model construction from text and graphics. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28, 1105-1126.

Hochstadt, J. (2009). Set-shifting and the on-line processing of relative clauses in Parkinson's disease: Results from a novel eye-tracking method. Cortex, 45, 991-1011.

Hodgson, T. L., Bajwa, A., Owen, A. M., & Kennard, C. (2000). The strategic control of gaze direction in the Tower of London Task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 894-907.

Hodgson, T. L., Chamberlain, M., Parris, B., James, M., Gutowski, N., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2007). The role of the ventrolateral frontal cortex in inhibitory oculomotor control. Brain, 130, 1525-1537.

Hodgson, T. L., Golding, C., Molyva, D., Rosenthal, C. R., Kennard, C. (2004). Eye movements during task switching: Reflexive, symbolic, and affective contributions to response selection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 318-330.

Hodgson, T. L., Mort, D., Chamberlain, M. M., Hutton, S. B., O'Neill, K. S., & Kennard, C. (2002). Orbitofrontal cortex mediates inhibition of return. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1891-1901.

Hodgson, T. L., Parris, B. A., Gregory, N. J., & Jarvis, T. (2009). The saccadic Stroop effect: Evidence for involuntary programming of eye movements by linguistic cues. Vision Research, 49, 569-574.

Hodgson, T. L., Sumner, P., Molyva, D., Sheridan, R., & Kennard, C. (2013). Learning and switching between stimulus-saccade associations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1350-1360.

Hodgson, T. L., Tiesman, B., Owen, A. M., & Kennard, O. C. (2002). Abnormal gaze strategies during problem solving in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 40, 411-422.

Hoffman, A. B., & Rehder, B. (2010). The costs of supervised classification: The effect of learning task on conceptual flexibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 319-340.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., & Duka, T. (2009). Detection versus sustained attention to drug cues have dissociable roles in mediating drug seeking behavior. Experimental & Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 21-30.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., & Duka, T. (2010). The associative basis of cue-elicited drug taking in humans. Psychopharmacology, 208, 337-351.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., Austin, A., Brown, C., & Duka, T. (2008). Attention and expectation in human predictive learning: The role of uncertainty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1658-1668.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., Hutton, S. B., Bamborough, H., & Duka, T. (2006). Contingency knowledge is necessary for learned motivated behaviour in humans: Relevance for addictive behaviour. Addiction, 101, 1153-1166.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., Hutton, S. B., Elbers, N., & Duka, T. (2006). Drug expectancy is necessary for stimulus control of human attention, instrumental drug-seeking behaviour and subjective pleasure. Psychopharmacology, 185, 495-504.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., Janowski, M., Nikitina, A., & Duka, T. (2008). The role of attentional bias in mediating human drug-seeking behaviour. Psychopharmacology, 201, 29-41.

Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., Wright, A., Kouvaraki, M., & Duka, T. (2007). The role of drug expectancy in the control of human drug seeking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 484-496.

Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2014). Semantic preview benefit during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 166-190.

Hohenstein, S., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2010). Semantic preview benefit in eye movements during reading: A parafoveal fast-priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1150-1170.

Holahan, A.-L., V., & O'Driscoll, G. A. (2005). Antisaccade and smooth pursuit performance in positive- and negative-symptom schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 76, 43-54.

Holcombe, A. O., & Chen, W.-Y. (2012). Exhausting attentional tracking resources with a single fast-moving object. Cognition, 123, 218-228.

Hollingworth, A. (2012). Task specificity and the influence of memory on visual search: Comment on Võ and Wolfe (2012). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 38, 1596-1603.

Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2009). The role of visual working memory in the control of gaze during visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 936-949.

Hollingworth, A., Matsukura, M., & Luck, S. J. (2013). Visual working memory modulates low-level saccade target selection: Evidence from rapidly generated saccades in the global effect paradigm. Journal of Vision, 13(13):4, 1–18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/13/4, doi:10.1167/13.13.4.

Hollingworth, A., Matsukura, M., & Luck, S. J. (2013). Visual working memory modulates rapid eye movements to simple onset targets. Psychological Science, 24, 790-796.

Holm, L., & Mäntylä, T. (2007). Memory for scenes: Refixations reflect retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1664-1674.

Holm, L., Engel, S., & Schrater, P. (2012). Object learning improves feature extraction but does not improve feature selection. PLoS ONE 7(12): e51325. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051325.

Holm, L., Eriksson, J., & Andersson, L. (2008). Looking as if you know: Systematic object inspection precedes object recognition. Journal of Vision, 8(4):14, 1-7, http://journalofvision.org/8/4/14/, doi:10.1167/8.4.14.

Holsinger, E. (2013). Representing idioms: Syntactic and contextual effects on idiom processing. Language and Speech, 56, 373-394.

Hong, L. E., Thaker, G. K., McMahon, R. P., Summerfelt, A., RachBeisel, J., Fuller, R. L., Wonodi, I., Buchanan, R. W., Myers, C., Heishman, S. J., Yang, J., Nye, A. (2011). Effects of moderate-dose rreatment with varenicline on neurobiological and cognitive biomarkers in smokers and nonsmokers with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 1195-1206.

Hong, L. E., Turano, K. A., O'Neill, H. B., Hao, L., Wonodi, I., McMahon, R. P., & Thaker, G. K. (2009). Is motion perception deficit in schizophrenia a consequence of eye-tracking abnormality? Biological Psychiatry, 65, 1079-1085.

Hong, L. E., Turano, K. A., O'Neill, H., Hao, L., Wonodi, I., McMahon, R. P., Elliott, A., & Thaker, G. K. (2008). Refining the predictive pursuit endophenotype in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 458-464.

Hong, S. L., & Beck, M. R. (2010). Uncertainty compensation in human attention: Evidence from response times and fixation durations. PLoS ONE 5(7): e11461. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011461.

Hoormann, J., Jainta, S., & Jaschinski, W. (2008). The effect of calibration errors on the accuracy of the eye movment recordings. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(2):3, 1-7.

Horsley, T. A., de Castro, B. O., & Van der Schoot, M. (2010). In the eye of the beholder: Eye-tracking assessment of social information processing in aggressive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 587-599.

Horstmann, A., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (2005). Target selection in eye-hand coordination: Do we reach to where we look or do we look to where we reach? Experimental Brain Research, 167, 187-195.

Hou, Y., & Liu, T. (2012). Neural correlates of object-based attentional selection in human cortex. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2916-2925.

Hout, M. C., & Goldinger, S. D. (2012). Incidental learning speeds visual search by lowering response thresholds, not by improving efficiency: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 90-112.

Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). The effect of items in working memory on the deployment of attention and the eyes during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 423-442.

Howard, I. S., Ingram, J. N., Franklin, D. W., & Wolpert, D. M. (2012). Gone in 0.6 Seconds: The encoding of motor memories depends on recent sensorimotor states. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 12756-12768.

Howard, L. R., Kumaran, D., Ólafsdóttir, H. F., & Spiers, H. J. (2011). Double dissociation between hippocampal and parahippocampal responses to object-background context and scene novelty. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 5253-5261.

Hsiao, J. H., & Cottrell, G. W. (2009). Not all visual expertise is holistic, but it may be leftist: The case of Chinese character recognition. Psychological Science, 20, 455-463.

Hsiao, J. H., & Liu, T. T. (2012). The optimal viewing position in face recognition. Journal of Vision, 12(2):22, 1-9, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/2/22, doi:10.1167/12.2.22.

Hsiao, J. H.-W., & Cottrell, G. (2008). Two fixations suffice in face recognition. Psychological Science, 19, 998-1006.

Hsiao, J.-Y., Chen, Y.-C., Spence, C., & Yeh, S.-L. (2012). Assessing the effects of audiovisual semantic congruency on the perception of a bistable figure. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 775-787.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2006). Illusory color mixing upon perceptual fading and filling-in does not result in 'forbidden colors'. Vision Research, 46, 2251-2258.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2006). Stimulus factors affecting illusory rebound motion. Vision Research, 46, 1924-1933.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2007). Grouping inhibits motion fading by giving rise to virtual trackable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 57-63.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2009). Feature mixing rather than feature replacement during perceptual filling-in. Vision Research, 49, 439-450.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2009). Microsaccade rate varies with subjective visibility during motion-induced blindness. PLoS ONE 4(4): e5163. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005163.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2009). Motion fading and the motion aftereffect share a common process of neural adaptation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 724-733.

Hsieh, P. -J., & Tse, P. U. (2010). BOLD signal in both ipsilateral and contralateral retinotopic cortex modulates with perceptual fading. PLoS ONE 5(3): e9638. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009638.

Hsieh, P. -J., Caplovitz , G. P., & Tse, P. U. (2005).Illusory rebound motion and the motion continuity heuristic. Vision Research, 45, 2972-2985.

Hsieh, P. -J., Caplovitz , G. P., & Tse, P. U. (2006).Illusory motion induced by the offset of stationary luminance-defined gradients. Vision Research, 46, 970-978.

Hsieh, Y., Boland, J. E., Zhang, Y., & Yan, M. (2009). Limited syntactic parallelism in Chinese ambiguity resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 1227-1264.

Hu, Y., & Walker, R. (2011). The neural basis of parallel saccade programming: A functional imaging (fMRI) study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3669-3680.

Huang, X., Cai, Z., & Chen, L. (2004). The effect of visual angle on the recognition of Chinese characters. Psychological Science (China), 27, 770-773.

Huang, X., Jing, J., Zou, X.-B., Wang, M.-L., Li, X.-H., & Lin, A.-H. (2008). Eye movements characteristics of Chinese dyslexic children in picture searching. Chinese Medical Journal, 121, 1617-1621.

Huang, Y.-F., & Kuo, F.-Y. (2011). An eye-tracking investigation of internet consumers' decision deliberateness. Internet Research, 21, 541-561.

Huang, Y.-F., & Kuo, F.-Y. (2012). How impulsivity affects consumer decision-making in e-commerce. Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 11, 582-590.

Huang, Y.-T., & Gordon, P. C. (2011). Distinguishing the time course of lexical and discourse processes through context, coreference, and quantified expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 966-978.

Huddleston, W. E., & DeYoe, E. A. (2008). The representation of spatial attention in human parietal cortex dynamically modulates with performance. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1272-1280.

Huddy, V. C., Hodgson, T. L., Kapasi, M., Mutsatsa, S. H., Harrison, I., Barnes, T. R. E., & Joyce, E. M. (2007). Gaze strategies during planning in first-episode psychosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 589-598.

Huddy, V. C., Hodgson, T. L., Ron, M. A., Barnes, T. R. E., & Joyce, E. M. (2011). Abnormal negative feedback processing in first episode schizophrenia: evidence from an oculomotor rule switching task. Psychological Medicine, 41, 1805-1814.

Huebner, G. M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Effects of viewing time, fixations, and viewing strategies on visual memory for briefly presented natural objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1398 - 1413.

Huestegge, L. & Radach, R. (2012). Visual and memory search in complex environments: determinants of eye movements and search performance. Ergonomics, 55, 1009-1027.

Huestegge, L. (2010). Effects of vowel length on gaze durations in silent and oral reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(5):5,1-18.

Huestegge, L., & Adam, J. J. (2011). Oculomotor interference during manual response preparation: evidence from the response-cueing paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 702-707.

Huestegge, L., & Bocianski, D. (2010). Effects of syntactic context on eye movements during reading. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 6, 79-87.

Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2009). Dual-task crosstalk between saccades and manual responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 352-362.

Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Crossmodal action selection: Evidence from dual-task compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 38, 493-501.

Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Fixation disengagement enhances peripheral perceptual processing: evidence for a perceptual gap effect. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 631-640.

Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2012). Eye movements as a gatekeeper for memorization: evidence for the persistence of attentional sets in visual memory search. Psychological Research, 76, 270-279.

Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2013). Constraints in task-set control: Modality dominance patterns among effector systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 633-637.

Huestegge, L., & Kreutzfeldt, M. (2012). Action effects in saccade control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 198-203.

Huestegge, L., & Philipp, A. M. (2011). Effects of spatial compatibility on integration processes in graph comprehension. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1903-1915.

Huestegge, L., Kunert, H.-J., & Radach, R. (2010). Long-term effects of cannabis on eye movement control in reading. Psychopharmacology, 209, 77-84.

Huestegge, L., Radach, R., & Kunert, H. J. (2009). Long-term effects of cannabis on oculomotor function in humans. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 23, 714-722.

Huestegge, L., Radach, R., Corbic, D., & Huestegge, S. M. (2009). Oculomotor and linguistic determinants of reading development: A longitudinal study. Vision Research, 49, 2948-2959.

Huestegge, L., Skottke, E.-M., Anders, S., Müsseler, J., & Debus, G. (2010). The development of hazard perception: Dissociation of visual orientation and hazard processing. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13, 1-8.

Huette, S., Kello, C.T., Rhodes. T,, & Spivey, M. J. (2013). Drawing from memory: Hand-eye coordination at multiple scales. PLoS ONE 8(3): e58464. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058464.

Huette, S., Winter, B., Matlock, T., & Spivey, M. (2012). Processing motion implied in language: eye-movement differences during aspect comprehension. Cognitive Processing, 13, S193-S197.

Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2005). Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96, B23-B32.

Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2007). Visual-shape competition during language-mediated attention is based on lexical input and not modulated by contextual appropriateness. Visual Cognition, 15, 985-1018.

Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2011). Looking at anything that is green when hearing "frog": How object surface colour and stored object colour knowledge influence language-mediated overt attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 122-145.

Huettig, F., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2008). When you name the pizza you look at the coin and the bread: Eye movements reveal semantic activation during word production. Memory & Cognition, 36, 341-360.

Huettig, F., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2010). Listening to yourself is like listening to others: External, but not internal, verbal self-monitoring is based on speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 347-374.

Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2007). The tug of war between phonological, semantic and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 460-482.

Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2011). The nature of the visual environment induces implicit biases during language-mediated visual search. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1068-1084.

Huettig, F., Chen, J., Bowerman, M., & Majid, A. (2010). Do language-specific categories shape conceptual processing? Mandarin classifier distinctions influence eye gaze behavior, but only during linguistic processing. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10, 39-58.

Huettig, F., Quinlan, P. T., McDonald, S. A., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2006). Models of high-dimensional semantic space predict language-mediated eye movements in the visual world. Acta Psychologica, 121, 65-80.

Hungr, C. J., & Hunt, A. R. (2012). Physical self-similarity enhances the gaze-cueing effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1250-1259.

Hunt, A. R., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). Looking ahead: The perceived direction of gaze shifts before the eyes move. Journal of Vision, 9(9):1, 1-7, http://journalofvision.org/9/9/1/, doi:10.1167/9.9.1.

Hunt, A. R., & Cavanagh, P. (2011). Remapped visual masking. Journal of Vision, 11(1):13, 1-8, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/1/13, doi:10.1167/11.1.13.

Hunt, A. R., & Klein, R. M. (2002). Eliminating the cost of task set reconfiguration. Memory & Cognition, 30, 529-539.

Hunt, A. R., Chapman, C. S., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Taking a long look at action and time perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34, 125-136.

Hunt, A. R., Cooper, R. M., & Hungr, C., & Kingstone, A. (2007). The effect of emotional faces on eye movements and attention. Visual Cognition, 15, 513-531.

Hunt, A. R., von Muhlenen, A., & Kingstone, A. (2007). The time course of attentional and oculomotor capture reveals a common cause. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 271-284.

Hurwitz, M., Valadao, D., & Danckert, J. (2011). Static versus dynamic judgments of spatial extent. Experimental Brain Research, 209, 271-286.

Husain, M., Mannan, S., Hodgson, T., Wojciulik, E., Driver, J., & Kennard, C.(2001). Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. Brain, 124, 941-952.

Husain, M., Parton, A., Hodgson, T. L., Mort, D., & Rees, G. (2003). Self-control during response conflict by human supplementary eye field. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 117-118.

Hutton, S. B., & Nolte, S. (2011). The effect of gaze cues on attention to print advertisements. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 887-892.

Hutton, S. B., & Tegally, D. (2005). The effects of dividing attention on smooth pursuit eye tracking. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 306-313.

Hutton, S. B., & Weekes, B. S. (2007). Low frequency rTMS over posterior parietal cortex impairs smooth pursuit eye tracking. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 195-200.

Hutzler, F., Fuchs, I., Gagl, B., Schuster, S., Richlan, F., Braun, M., & Hawelka, S. (2013). Parafoveal X-masks interfere with foveal word recognition: evidence from fixation-related brain potentials. Frontiers in System Neuroscience, 7:33. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00033.

Hutzler, F., Kronbichler, M., Jacobs, A. M., & Wimmer, H. (2006). Perhaps correlational but not causal: No effect of dyslexic readers' magnocellular system on their eye movements during reading. Neuropsychologia, 44, 637-648.

Hwang, A. D., Higgins, E. C., & Pomplun, M. (2009). A model of top-down attentional control during visual search in complex scenes. Journal of Vision, 9(5):25, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/9/5/25/, doi:10.1167/9.5.25.

Hwang, A. D., Wang, H.-C., & Pomplun, M. (2011). Semantic guidance of eye movements in real-world scenes. Vision Research, 51, 1192-1205.

Hyrskykari, A. (2005). Utilizing eye movements: Overcoming inaccuracy while tracking the focus of attention during reading. Computers in Human Behavior, 22, 657-671.

Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2004). Do frequency characteristics of non-fixated words influence the processing of non-fixated words during reading? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 104-127.

Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2011). Optimal viewing position effects in reading Finnish. Vision Research, 51, 1279-1287.

Hyönä, J., & Häikiö, T. (2005). Is emotional content obtained from parafoveal words during reading? An eye movement analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 475-483.

Hyönä, J., & Koivisto, M. (2006). The role of eye movements in lateralised word recognition. Laterality, 11, 155-169.

Hyönä, J., & Laine, M. (2002). A morphological effect obtains for isolated words but not for words in sentence context. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 417-433.

Hyönä, J., & Lorch, R. F., Jr. (2004). Effects of topic headings on text processing: Evidence from adult readers' eye fixation patterns. Learning and Instruction, 14, 131-152.

Hyönä, J., & Nurminen, A. M. (2006). Do adult readers know how they read? Evidence from eye movement patterns and verbal reports. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 31-50.

Hyönä, J., & Pollatsek, A. (1998). Reading Finnish compound words: Eye fixations are affected by component morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1612-1627.

Hyönä, J., & Vainio, S. (2001). Reading morphologically complex clause structures in Finnish. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 451-474.

Hyönä, J., Bertram, R., & Pollatsek, A. (2004). Are long compound words identified serially via their constituents? Evidence from an eye-movement contingent display change study. Memory & Cognition, 32, 523-532.

Hyönä, J., Lorch, R. F. Jr., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2002). Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: Evidence from eye fixation patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 44-55.

Häikiö, T., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2011). The development of whole-word representations in compound word processing: Evidence from eye fixation patterns of elementary school children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 533-551.

Häikiö, T., Bertram, R., Hyönä, J., & Niemi, P. (2009). Development of the letter identity span in reading: Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 167-181.

Häikiö, T., Hyönä, J., Bertram, R. (2010). Development of parafoveal processing within and across words in reading: Evidence from the boundary paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1982-1998.

Höfler, M., Gilchrist, I. D., & Körner, C. (2011). Inhibition of return functions within but not across searches. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1385-1397.

Hübner, J., Sprenger, A., Klein, C., Hagenah, J., Rambold, H., Zühlke, C., Kömpf, D., Rolfs, A., Kimmig, H., & Helmchen, C. (2007). Eye movement abnormalities in spinocerebellar ataxia type 17 (SCA17). Neurology, 69, 1160-1168.

Hügli, H., Jost, T., & Ouerhani, N. (2007). Model performance for visual attention in real 3D color scenes. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 3562, 469-478.

Back To Top

I

Ikeda, H., Blake, R., & Watanabe, K. (2005). Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor. Vision Research, 45, 1935-1943.

Imaruoka, T., Yanagida, T., & Miyauchi, S. (2003). Attentional set for external information activates the right intraparietal area. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 199-209.

Indovina, I., Maffei, V., Bosco, G., Zago, M., Macaluso, E., & Lacquaniti, F. (2005). Representation of visual gravitational motion in the human vestibular cortex. Science, 308, 416-419.

Indurkhya, B., & Ojha, A. (2013). An empirical study on the role of perceptual similarity in visual metaphors and creativity. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 233-253.

Inhoff, A. W., Connine, C., & Radach, R. (2002). A contingent speech technique in eye movement research on reading. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 34, 471-480.

Inhoff, A. W., Connine, C., Eiter, B., Radach, R., & Heller, D. (2004). Phonological representation of words in working memory during sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 320-325.

Inhoff, A. W., Eiter, B. M., & Radach, R. (2005). Time course of linguistic information extraction from consecutive words during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 979-995.

Inhoff, A. W., Greenberg, S. N., Solomon, M., & Wang, C.-A. (2009). Word integration and regression programming during reading: A test of the E-Z reader 10 model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 1571-1584.

Inhoff, A. W., Radach, R., Eiter, B., & Juhasz, B. (2003). Distinct subsystems for the parafoveal processing of spatial and linguistic information during eye fixations in reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 803-827.

Inhoff, A. W., Seymour, B. A., & Radach, r. (2012). Use of colour for language processing during reading. Visual Cognition, 20, 1254-1265.

Inhoff, A. W., Seymour, B. A., Schad, D., & Greenberg, S. (2010). The size and direction of saccadic curvatures during reading. Vision Research, 50, 1117-1130.

Inhoff, A. W., Solomon, M. S., Seymour, B. A., & Radach, R. (2008). Eye position changes during reading fixations are spatially selective. Vision Research, 48, 1027-1039.

Inhoff, A. W., Solomon, M., Radach, R., & Seymour, B. A. (2011). Temporal dynamics of the eye-voice span and eye movement control during oral reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 543-558.

Inhoff, A. W., Starr, M. S., Solomon, M., & Placke, L. (2008). Eye movements during the reading of compound words and the influence of lexeme meaning. Memory & Cognition, 36, 675-687.

Intraub, H., & Dickinson, C. A. (2008). False memory 1/20th of a second later: What the early onset of boundary extension reveals about perception. Psychological Science, 19, 1007-1014.

Iordanescu, L., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2009). Demand-based dynamic distribution of attention and monitoring of velocities during multiple-object tracking. Journal of Vision, 9(4):1, 1-12, http://journalofvision.org/9/4/1/, doi:10.1167/9.4.1.

Iordanescu, L., Grabowecky, M., Franconeri, S., Theeuwes, J., & Suzuki, S. (2010). Characteristic sounds make you look at the targets faster in visual search. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 1736-1741.

Irmen, L. (2007). What's in a (role) name? Formal and conceptual aspects of comprehending personal nouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 431-456.

Irmen, L., & Schumann, E. (2011). Processing grammatical gender of role nouns: Further evidence from eye movements. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 998-1014.

Irwin, D. E. (2011). Where does attention go when you blink? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1374-1384.

Irwin, D. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). Visual marking across eye blinks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 128-134.

Irwin, D. E., & Thomas, L. E. (2007). The effect of saccades on number processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 450-458.

Irwin, D. E., Colcombe, A. M., Kramer, A. F., & Hahn, S. (2000). Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance, and color singletons. Vision Research, 40, 1443-1458.

Isaacs, A. M., & Watson, D. G. (2010). Accent detection is a slippery slope: Direction and rate of F0 change drives listeners' comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 1178-1200.

Isham, E.A., & Geng, J.J. (2013). Looking time predicts choice but not aesthetic value. PLoS ONE 8(8): e71698. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0071698.

Issa, E. B., & DiCarlo, J. J. (2012). Precedence of the eye region in neural processing of faces. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 16666-16682.

Issen, L. A., & Knill, D. C. (2012). Decoupling eye and hand movement control: Visual short-term memory influences reach planning more than saccade planning. Journal of Vision, 12(1):3, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/1/3, doi:10.1167/12.1.3.

Itier, R. J., Villate, C., & Ryan, J. D. (2007). Eyes always attract attention but gaze orienting is task-dependent: Evidence from eye movement monitoring. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1019-1028.

Ito, J., Nikolaev, A. R., Luman, M., Aukes, M. F., & Nakatani, C. (2003). Perceptual switching, eye movements, and the bus paradox. Perception, 32, 681-698.

Iyilikci, O., Becker, C., Güntürkün, O., & Amado, S. (2010). Visual processing asymmetries in change detection. Perception, 39, 761-769.

Back To Top

J

Jack, R. E., Blais, C., Scheepers, C., Schyns, P. G., & Caldara, R. (2009). Cultural confusions show that facial expressions are not universal. Current Biology, 19, 1543-1548.

Jackson, C. N., Dussias, P. E., & Hristova, A. (2012). Using eye-tracking to study the on-line processing of case-marking information among intermediate L2 learners of German. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 50, 101-133.

Jackson, S., Cummins, F., & Brady, N. (2008). Rapid perceptual switching of a reversible biological figure. PLoS ONE 3(12): e3982. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003982

Jacob, M., & Hochstein, S. (2009). Comparing eye movements to detected vs. undetected target stimuli in an Identity Search task. Journal of Vision, 9(5):20, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/9/5/20/, doi:10.1167/9.5.20.

Jacob, M., & Hochstein, S. (2010). Graded recognition as a function of the number of target fixations. Vision Research, 50, 107-117.

Jacobs, J. B., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2010). Extending the expanded nystagmus acuity function for vertical and multiplanar data. Vision Research, 50, 271-278.

Jacobs, J. B., Dell'Osso, L. F., Hertle, R. W., Acland, G. M., & Bennett, J. (2006). Eye movement recordings as an effectiveness indicator of gene therapy in RPE65-deficient canines: Implications for the ocular motor system. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 47, 2865-2875.

Jacobs, J. B., Dell'Osso, L. F., Wang, Z. I., Acland, G., & Bennett, J. (2009). Using the NAFX to measure the effectiveness over time of gene therapy in canine LCA. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 50, 4685-4692.

Jacobs, R. H. A. H., Renken, R., Thumfart, S., & Cornelissen, F. W. (2010). Different judgments about visual textures invoke different eye movement patterns. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(4):2, 1-13.

Jain, A., Fuller, S., & Backus, B. T. (2010). Absence of cue-recruitment for extrinsic signals: Sounds, spots, and wwirling dots fail to influence perceived 3D rotation direction after training. PLoS ONE 5(10): e13295. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013295.

Jain, S., Proudlock, F., Constantinescu, C. S., & Gottlob, I. (2002). Combined pharmacologic and surgical approach to acquired nystagmus due to multiple sclerosis. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 134, 780-782.

Jainta, S., & Jaschinski, W. (2010). "Trait" and "state" aspects of fixation disparity during reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3: 3(3):1, 1-13.

Jainta, S., & Jaschinski, W. (2012). Individual differences in binocular coordination are uncovered by directly comparing monocular and binocular reading conditions. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 53, 5762-5769.

Jainta, S., Dehnert, A., Heinrich, S. P., & Jaschinski, W. (2011). Binocular coordination during reading of blurred and nonblurred text. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 52, 9416-9424.

Jainta, S., Hoormann, J., & Jaschinski, W. (2007). Objective and subjective measures of vergence step responses. Vision Research, 47, 3238-3246.

Jainta, S., Hoormann, J., & Jaschinski, W. (2009). Accommodation modulates the individual difference between objective and subjective measures of the final convergence step response. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 29, 162-172.

Jainta, S., Hoormann, J., Kloke, W. B., & Jaschinski, W. (2010). Binocularity during reading fixations: properties of the minimum fixation disparity. Vision Research, 50, 1775-1785.

Jainta, S., Jaschinski, W., & Wilkins, A. J. (2010). Periodic letter strokes within a word affect fixation disparity during reading. Journal of Vision, 10(13):2, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/13/2, doi:10.1167/10.13.2.

Jainta, S., Vernet, M., Yang, Q., & Kapoula, Z. (2011). The pupil reflects motor preparation for saccades - even before the eye starts to move. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 5:97. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00097.

Jansen, L., Onat, S., & König, P. (2009). Influence of disparity on fixation and saccades in free viewing of natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 9(1):29, 1-19, http://journalofvision.org/9/1/29/, doi:10.1167/9.1.29.

Jansonius, N. M., van der Vliet, T. A., Cornelissen, F. W., Pott, J. W. R., & Kooijman, A. C. (2001). A girl without a chiasm: Electrophysiologic and MRI evidence for the absence of crossing optic nerve fibers in a girl with a congenital nystagmus. Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology, 21, 26-29.

Janssen, M., de Vries, J., Ischebeck, B. K., Frens, M. A., & van der Geest, J. N. (2013). Small effects of neck torsion on healthy human voluntary eye movements. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 113, 3049-3057.

Janssen, P., Srivastava, S., Ombelet, S., & Orban, G. A. (2008). Coding of shape and position in macaque lateral intraparietal area. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 6679-6690.

Jarosz, A. F., & Wiley, J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict RAPM performance? A possible role of distraction. Intelligence, 40, 427-438.

Jaschinski, W., Jainta, S., & Hoormann, J. (2007). Comparison of shutter glasses and mirror stereoscope for measuring dynamic and static vergence. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(2):5, 1-7.

Jaschinski, W., Jainta, S., & Kloke, W. B. (2010). Objective vs subjective measures of fixation disparity for short and long fixation periods. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 30, 379 - 390.

Jaschinski, W., Jainta, S., Hoormann, J., & Walper, N. (2007). Objective vs subjective measurements of dark vergence. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 27, 85-92.

Jenkinson, N., Brittain, J.-S., Hicks, S.L., Kennard, C., & Aziz, T.Z. (2012). On the origin of oscillopsia during pedunculopontine stimulation. Stereotact and Functional Neurosurgy, 90, 124-129.

Jerde, T. A., Merriam, E. P., Riggall, A. C., Hedges, J. H., & Curtis, C. E. (2012). Prioritized maps of space in human frontoparietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 17382-17390.

Jian, Y.-C., Chen, M.-L., & Ko, H.-W. (2013). Context effects in processing of Chinese academic words: An eye-tracking investigation. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 403-413.

Jin, Z., Reeves, A., Watamaniuk, S. N. J., & Heinen, S. J. (2013). Shared attention for smooth pursuit and saccades. Journal of Vision, 13(4):7, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/4/7, doi:10.1167/13.4.7.

Jingling, L., Tang, D.-H., & Tseng, C.-H. (2013). Salient collinear grouping diminishes local salience in visual search: An eye movement study. Journal of Vision, 13(12):6, 1-10, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/12/6, doi:10.1167/13.12.6.

Johnson, A., & Gurnsey, R. (2010). Size scaling compensates for sensitivity loss produced by a simulated central scotoma in a shape-from-texture task. Journal of Vision, 10(12):18, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/12/18, doi:10.1167/10.12.18.

Johnson, B. P., Rinehart, N. J., Papadopoulos, N., Tonge, B., Millist, L., White, O., & Fielding, J. (2012). A closer look at visually guided saccades in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6:99. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00099.

Johnson, B. P., Rinehart, N. J., White, O., Millist, L., & Fielding, J. (2013). Saccade adaptation in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Neuroscience, 243, 76-87.

Johnson, M. L., Lowder, M. W., & Gordon, P. C. (2011). The sentence-composition effect: Processing of complex sentences depends on the configuration of common versus unusual noun phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 707-724.

Johnson, R. L. & Eisler, M. E. (2012). The importance of the first and last letter in words during sentence reading. Acta Psychologica,141, 336-351.

Johnson, R. L. (2009). The quiet clam is quite calm: Transposed-letter neighborhood effects on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 943-969.

Johnson, R. L., & Rayner, K. (2007). Top-down and bottom-up effects in pure alexia: Evidence from eye movements. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2246-2257.

Jolij, J., Scholte, H. S., van Gaal, S., Hodgson, T. L., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2011). Act quickly, decide later: Long-latency visual processing underlies perceptual decisions but not reflexive behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3734-3745.

Jonas, C. N., Taylor, A. J. G., Hutton, S., Weiss, P. H., & Ward, J. (2011). Visuo-spatial representations of the alphabet in synaesthetes and non-synaesthetes. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 302-322.

Jones, J. L., & Kaschak, M. P. (2012). Global statistical learning in a visual search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 152-160.

Jones, M. W., Ashby, J., & Branigan, H. P. (2013). Dyslexia and fluency: Parafoveal and foveal influences on rapid automatized naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 554-567.

Jones, M. W., Branigan, H. P., Hatzidaki, A., & Obregón, M. (2010). Is the 'naming' deficit in dyslexia a misnomer? Cognition, 116, 56-70.

Jones, M. W., Obregón, M., Kelly, M. L., & Branigan, H. P. (2008). Elucidating the component processes involved in dyslexic and non-dyslexic reading fluency: An eye-tracking study. Cognition, 109, 389-407.

Jones, S. A. H., & Henriques, D. Y. P. (2010). Memory for proprioceptive and multisensory targets is partially coded relative to gaze. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3782-3792.

Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2011). Independent allocation of attention to eye and hand targets in coordinated eye-hand movements. Psychological Science, 22, 339-347.

Jonikaitis, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). Dissociating oculomotor contributions to spatial and feature-based selection. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 1525-1534.

Jonikaitis, D., Schubert, T., & Deubel, H. (2010). Preparing coordinated eye and hand movements: Dual-task costs are not attentional. Journal of Vision, 10(14):23, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/23, doi:10.1167/10.14.23.

Jonikaitis, D., Szinte, M., Rolfs, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2013). Allocation of attention across saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 109, 1425-1434.

Joosten, F., De Sutter, G., Drieghe, D., Grondelaers, S., Hartsuiker, R., & Speelman, D. (2007). Dutch collective nouns and conceptual profiling. Linguistics, 45, 85-132.

Jordan, T. R., McGowan, V. A., & Paterson, K. B. (2012). Reading with a filtered fovea: The influence of visual quality at the point of fixation during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1078-1084.

Jordan, T. R., McGowan, V. A., & Paterson, K. B. (2013). What's left? An eye movement study of the influence of interword spaces to the left of fixation during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 551-557.

Joseph, H. S. S. L., Nation, K., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Using eye movements to investigate word frequency effects in children's sentence reading. School Psychology Review, 42, 207-222.

Joseph, H.S.S.L., & Liversedge, S.P. (2013). Children's and adults' on-line processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences during reading. PLoS ONE 8(1) e54141. doi10.1371journal.pone.0054141.

Josipovic, Z., Dinstein, I., Weber, J., & Heeger, D. J. (2012). Influence of meditation on anticorrelated networks in the brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5:183. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00183.

Jost, T., Ouerhani, N., von Wartburg, R., Müri, R., & Hügli, H. (2005). Assessing the contribution of color in visual attention. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 100, 107-123.

Ju, M., & Luce, P. A. (2004). Falling on sensitive ears: Constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science, 15, 314-318.

Juan, C.-H., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2008). Segregation of visual selection and saccades in human frontal eye fields. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2410-2415.

Juhasz, B. J., & Berkowitz, R. N. (2011). Effects of morphological families on English compound word recognition: a multitask investigation. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 653-682.

Juhasz, B. J., Gullick, M. M., & Sheslar, L. W. (2011). The effects of age-of-acquisition on ambiguity resolution: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Eye Movement Research 4(1):4, 1-14.

Juravle, G., & Deubel, H. (2009). Action preparation enhances the processing of tactile targets. Experimental Brain Research, 198, 301-311.

Jäkel, F., & Wichmann, F. A. (2006). Spatial four-alternative forced-choice method is the preferred psychophysical method for naïve observers. Journal of Vision, 6(11):13, 1307-1322, http://journalofvision.org/6/11/13/, doi:10.1167/6.11.13.

Järvikivi, J., van Gompel, R. P. G., Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2005). Ambiguous pronoun resolution: Contrasting the first-mention and subject-preference accounts. Psychological Science, 16, 260-264.

Järvilehto, T., Nurkkala, V.-M., & Koskela, K. (2009). The role of anticipation in reading. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17, 509-526.

Back To Top

K

Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2005). Perspective effects on expository text comprehension: Evidence from think-aloud protocols, eyetracking, and recalls. Discourse Processes, 40, 239-257.

Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2007). Perspective effects in repeated reading: An eye movement study. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1323-1336.

Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2007). Strategy use in the reading span test: An analysis of eye movements and reported encoding strategies. Memory, 15, 634-646.

Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2008). Perspective-driven text comprehension. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 319-334.

Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2010). Task effects on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1561-1566.

Kaakinen, J. K., Hyönä, J., & Keenan, J. M. (2002). Perspective effects on online text processing. Discourse Processes, 33, 159-173.

Kaakinen, J. K., Hyönä, J., & Keenan, J. M. (2003): How prior knowledge, WMC, and relevance of information affect eye fixations in expository text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29, 447-457.

Kaakinen, J. K., Hyönä, J., & Viljanen, M. (2011). Influence of a psychological perspective on scene viewing and memory for scenes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1372-1387.

Kabanarou, S. A., & Rubin, G. S. (2006). Reading with central scotomas: Is there a binocular gain? Optometry & Vision Science, 83, 789-796.

Kabanarou, S. A., Bellmann, C., Crossland, M. D., Rees, A., Feely, M., Culham, L. E., & Rubin, G. S. (2005). Binocular versus monocular viewing in patients with age-related macular degeneration. International Congress Series, 1282, 613-616.

Kabanarou, S. A., Crossland, M. D., Bellmann, C., Rees, A., Culham, L. E., & Rubin, G. S. (2006). Gaze changes with binocular versus monocular viewing in age-related macular degeneration. Ophthalmology, 113, 2251-2258.

Kaiser, E. (2011). Focusing on pronouns: Consequences of subjecthood, pronominalisation, and contrastive focus. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1625-1666.

Kaiser, M., & Lappe, M. (2004). Perisaccadic mislocalization orthogonal to saccade direction. Neuron, 41, 293-300.

Kalisvaart, J. P., Rampersad, S. M., & Goossens, J. (2011). Binocular onset rivalry at the time of saccades and stimulus jumps. PLoS ONE 6(6): e20017. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020017.

Kalla, R., Muggleton, N. G., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2009). Human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in visual search for conjunctions but not features: a theta TMS study. Cortex, 45, 1085-1090.

Kalla, R., Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C.-H., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2008). The timing of the involvement of the frontal eye fields and posterior parietal cortex in visual search. NeuroReport, 19, 1067-1071.

Kallio, S., Hyönä, J., Revonsuo, A., Sikka, P., & Nummenmaa, L. (2011). The Existence of a Hypnotic State Revealed by Eye Movements. PLoS ONE 6(10): e26374. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026374.

Kalénine, S., Mirman, D., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2012). A combination of thematic and similarity-based semantic processes confers resistance to deficit following left hemisphere stroke. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2012;6:106.

Kalénine, S., Mirman, D., Middleton, E. L., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2012). Temporal dynamics of activation of thematic and functional knowledge during conceptual processing of manipulable artifacts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1274-1295.

Kamboj, S. K., Massey-Chase, R., Rodney, L., Das, R., Almahdi, B., Curran, H. V., & Morgan, C. J. A. (2011). Changes in cue reactivity and attentional bias following experimental cue exposure and response prevention: a laboratory study of the effects of d-cycloserine in heavy drinkers. Psychopharmacology, 217, 25-37.

Kamide, Y. (2012). Learning individual talkers’ structural preferences. Cognition, 124, 66-71.

Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133-156.

Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C., &. Altmann, G. T. M. (2003). Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 37-55.

Kamienkowski, J. E., Ison, M. J., Quiroga, R. Q., & Sigman, M. (2012). Fixation-related potentials in visual search: A combined EEG and eye tracking study. Journal of Vision, 12(7):4, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/7/4, doi:10.1167/12.7.4.

Kamienkowski, J. E., Navajas, J., & Sigman, M. (2012). Eye movements blink the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 555-560.

Kaminiarz, A., Krekelberg, B., & Bremmer, F. (2007). Localization of visual targets during optokinetic eye movements. Vision Research, 47, 869-878.

Kaminiarz, A., Krekelberg, B., & Bremmer, F. (2008). Expansion of visual space during optokinetic afternystagmus (OKAN). Journal of Neurophysiology, 99, 2470-2478.

Kaminiarz, A., Königs, K., & Bremmer, F. (2009). Task influences on the dynamic properties of fast eye movements. Journal of Vision, 9(13):1, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/9/13/1/, doi:10.1167/9.13.1.

Kaminiarz, A., Königs, K., & Bremmer, F. (2009). The main sequence of human Optokinetic Afternystagmus (OKAN). Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 2889-2897.

Kamke, M. R., Hall, M. G., Lye, H. F., Sale, M. V., Fenlon, L. R., Carroll, T. J., Riek, S., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Visual attentional load influences plasticity in the human motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 7001-7008.

Kamke, M. R., Vieth, H. E., Cottrell, D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Parietal disruption alters audiovisual binding in the sound-induced flash illusion. Neuroimage, 62, 1334-1341.

Kan, J. Y., Niel, U., Dorris, M. C. (2012). Evidence for a link between the experiential allocation of saccade preparation and visuospatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 1413-1420.

Kanai, R., Muggleton, N., & Walsh, V. (2012). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the frontal eye fields during pro- and antisaccade Tasks. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 3, 45.

Kanai, R., Sheth, B. R., & Shimojo, S. (2007). Dynamical evolution of motion perception. Vision Research, 7, 937-945.

Kanai, R., van der Geest, J., & Frens, M. (2003). Inhibition of saccade initiation by preceding smooth pursuit. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 300-307.

Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Wang, J. T.-Y., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). The wick in the candle of learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory. Psychological Science, 20, 963-973.

Kanonidou, E., Proudlock, F. A., & Gottlob, I. (2010). Reading strategies in mild to moderate strabismic amblyopia: an eye movement investigation. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 51, 3502-3508.

Kapoula, Z., Yang, Q., Bonnet, A., Bourtoire, P., & Sandretto, J. (2010). EMDR effects on pursuit eye movements. PLoS ONE 5(5): e10762. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010762.

Kapoula, Z., Yang, Q., Sabbah, N., & Vernet, M. (2011). Different effects of double-pulse TMS of the posterior parietal cortex on reflexive and voluntary saccades. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 5:114. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00114.

Kapoula, Z., Yang, Q., Vernet, M., Bonfils, P., & Londero, A. (2010). Eye movement abnormalities in somatic tinnitus: Fixation, smooth pursuit and optokinetic nystagmus. Auris Nasus Larynx, 37, 314-321.

Kapoula, Z., Yang, Q., Vernet, M., Dieudonné, B., Greffard, S., & Verny, M. (2010). Spread deficits in initiation, speed and accuracy of horizontal and vertical automatic saccades in dementia with Lewy bodies. Frontiers in Dementia, doi:10.3389/fneur.2010.00138.

Kar, K., & Krekelberg, B. (2012). Transcranial electrical stimulation over visual cortex evokes phosphenes with a retinal origin. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 2173-2178.

Kasisopa, B., Reilly, R. G., Luksaneeyanawin, S., & Burnham, D. (2013). Eye movements while reading an unspaced writing system: The case of Thai. Vision Research, 86, 71-80.

Kaspar, K., & König, P. (2011). Overt attention and context factors: The impact of repeated presentations, image type, and individual motivation. PLoS ONE 6(7): e21719. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021719.

Kaspar, K., & König, P. (2011). Viewing behavior and the impact of low-level image properties across repeated presentations of complex scenes. Journal of Vision, 11(13):26, 1-29, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/13/26.

Kaspar, K., Hloucal, T.-M., Kriz, J., Canzler, S., Gameiro, R.R., Krapp, V., & König, P. (2013). Emotions' impact on viewing behavior under natural conditions. PLoS ONE 8(1): e52737. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052737.

Katsika, A., Braze, D., Deo, A., & Piñango, M. M. (2012). Complement Coercion: Distinguishing between type-shifting and pragmatic inferencing. The Mental Lexicon, 7, 58-76.

Kawasaki, K., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2008). Learning to recognize visual objects with microstimulation in inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 197-211.

Ke, S. R., Lam, J., Pai, D. K., & Spering, M. (2013). Directional asymmetries in human smooth pursuit eye movements. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54, 4409-4421.

Keating, G. D. (2009). Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59, 503-535.

Keehn, B., Brenner, L. A., Ramos, A. I., Lincoln, A. J., Marshall, S. P., & Müller, R. -A. (2009). Eye-movement patterns during an embedded figures test in children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 383-387.

Kelders, W. P. A., Kleinrensink, G. J., van Der Geest, J. N., Schipper, I. B., Feenstra, L., De Zeeuw, C. I., & Frens, M. A. (2005). The cervico-ocular reflex is increased in Whiplash injury patients. Journal of Neurotrauma, 22, 133-137.

Kelders, W. P. A., Kleinrensink, G. J., van der Geest, J. N., Feenstra, L., de Zeeuw, C. I., & Frens, M. A. (2003). Compensatory increase of the cervico-ocular reflex with age in healthy humans. Journal of Physiology, 553, 311-317.

Keller, E. L., Lee, K.-M., Park, S.-W., & Hill, J. A. (2008). Effect of inactivation of the cortical frontal eye field on saccades generated in a choice response paradigm. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 2726-2737.

Kelly, D. J., Jack, R. E., Miellet, S., De Luca, E., Foreman, K., & Caldara, R. (2011). Social experience does not abolish cultural diversity in eye movements. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:95. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00095.

Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Rodger, H., Miellet, S., Ge, L., & Caldara, R. (2011). Developing cultural differences in face processing. Developmental Science, 14, 1176-1184.

Kelly, D. J., Miellet, S., & Caldara, R. (2010). Culture shapes eye movements for visually homogeneous objects. Frontiers in Psychology, 1:6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00006.

Kelly, S. P., Foxe, J. J., Newman, G., & Edelman, J. A. (2010). Prepare for conflict: EEG correlates of the anticipation of target competition during overt and covert shifts of visual attention. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1690-1700.

Kelty-Stephen, D. G., Palatinus, K., Saltzman, E., & Dixon, J. A. (2013). A tutorial on multifractality, cascades, and interactivity for empirical time series in ecological science. Ecological Psychology, 25, 1-62.

Kemner, C., van Ewijk, L., van Engeland, H., & Hooge, I. (2008). Eye movements during visual search tasks indicate enhanced stimulus discriminability in subjects with PDD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 553-557.

Kennedy, D. P., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Impaired fixation to eyes following amygdala damage arises from abnormal bottom-up attention. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3392-3398.

Keren, A. S., Yuval-Greenberg, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2010). Saccadic spike potentials in gamma-band EEG: characterization, detection and suppression. NeuroImage, 49, 2248-2263.

Kerrigan, S. J., & Soechting, J. F. (2007). Anisotropies in the gain of smooth pursuit during two-dimensional tracking as probed by brief perturbations. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 435-448.

Kerzel, D. (2003). Mental extrapolation of target position is strongest with weak motion signals and motor responses. Vision Research, 43, 2623-2635.

Kerzel, D. (2004). Attentional load modulates mislocalization of moving stimuli, but does not eliminate the error. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 848-853.

Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2003). Neuronal processing delays are compensated in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system. Current Biology, 13, 1975-1978.

Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2005). Motion-induced illusory displacement reexamined: differences between perception and action? Experimental Brain Research, 162, 191-201.

Kerzel, D., & Schönhammer, J. (2013). Salient stimuli capture attention and action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1633-1643.

Kerzel, D., & Ziegler, N. E. (2005). Visual short-term memory during smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 354-372.

Kerzel, D., Aivar, M. P., Ziegler, N. E., & Brenner. E. (2006). Mislocalization of flashes during smooth pursuit hardly depends on the lighting conditions. Vision Research, 46, 1145-1154.

Kerzel, D., Born, S., & Souto, D. (2009). Smooth pursuit eye movements and perception share target selection, but only some central resources. Behavioural Brain Research, 201, 66-73.

Kerzel, D., Born, S., & Souto, D. (2010). Inhibition of steady-state smooth pursuit and catch-up saccades by abrupt visual and auditory onsets. Journal of Neurophysioloy, 104, 2573-2585.

Kerzel, D., Gauch, A., & Ulmann, B. (2008). Local motion inside an object affects pointing less than smooth pursuit. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 187-195.

Kerzel, D., Souto, D., & Ziegler, N. E. (2008). Effects of attention shifts to stationary objects during steady-state smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Research, 48, 958-969.

Kessels, L. T. E., & Ruiter, R. A. C. (2012). Eye movement responses to health messages on cigarette packages. BMC Public Health, 12:352. http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2458/12/352.

Ketcham, C. J., Dounskaia, N. V., & Stelmach, G. E. (2006). The role of vision in the control of continuous multijoint movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 29-44.

Ketcham, C. J., Hodgson, T. L., Kennard, C., & Stelmach, G. E. (2003). Memory-motor transformations are impaired in Parkinson's disease. Experimental Brain Research, 149, 30-39.

Khalid, S., & Ansorge, U. (2013). The Simon effect of spatial words in eye movements: Comparison of vertical and horizontal effects and of eye and finger responses. Vision Research, 86, 6-14.

Khalid, S., Finkbeiner, M., König, P., & Ansorge, U. (2013). Subcortical human face processing? Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 989-100.

Khan, A. Z., Heinen, S. J., & McPeek, R. M. (2010). Attentional cueing at the saccade goal, not at the target location, facilitates saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 5481-5488.

Khan, A. Z., Lefèvre, P., Heinen, S. J., & Blohm, G. (2010). The default allocation of attention is broadly ahead of smooth pursuit. Journal of Vision, 10(13):7, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/13/7, doi:10.1167/10.13.7.

Khan, A. Z., Song, J.-H., & McPeek, R. M. (2011). The eye dominates in guiding attention during simultaneous eye and hand movements. Journal of Vision, 11(1):9, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/1/9, doi:10.1167/11.1.9.

Khan, M., & Daneman, M. (2011). How Readers Spontaneously Interpret Man-Suffix Words: Evidence from Eye Movements. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40, 351-366.

Khan, R. A., Meyer, A., Konik, H., & Bouakaz, S. (2013). Framework for reliable, real-time facial expression recognition for low resolution images. Pattern Recognition Letters, 34, 1159-1168.

Khawaja, F. A., Liu, D. L., & Pack, C. C. (2013). Responses of MST neurons to plaid stimuli. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 63-74.

Khawaja, F. A., Tsui, J. M. G., & Pack, C. C. (2009). Pattern motion selectivity of spiking outputs and local field potentials in macaque visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 13702-13709.

Khayat, P. S., Niebergall, R., & Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2010). Attention differentially modulates similar neuronal responses evoked by varying contrast and direction stimuli in Area MT. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 2188-2197.

Khayat, P. S., Niebergall, R., & Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2010). Frequency-dependent attentional modulation of local field potential signals in macaque area MT. Jource of Neuroscience, 30, 7037-7048.

Kibbe, M. M., & Kowler, E. (2011). Visual search for category sets: Tradeoffs between exploration and memory. Journal of Vision, 11(3):14, 1-21, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/3/14, doi:10.1167/11.3.14.

Kienzle, W., Franz, M. O., Schölkopf, B., & Wichmann, F. A. (2009). Center-surround patterns emerge as optimal predictors for human saccade targets. Journal of Vision, 9(5):7, 1-15, http://journalofvision.org/9/5/7/, doi:10.1167/9.5.7.

Kienzle, W., Schölkopf, B., Wichmann, F. A., & Franz, M. O. (2007). How to find interesting locations in video: A Spatiotemporal interest point detector learned from human eye movements. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4713, 405-414.

Kietzmann, T. C., Geuter, S., & König, P. (2011). Overt visual attention as a causal factor of perceptual awareness. PLoS ONE 6(7): e22614. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022614.

Kilpelainen, M., Olivers, C. N. L., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). The eyes like their targets on a stable background. Journal of Vision, 13(6):5, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/6/5, doi:10.1167/13.6.5.

Kim, B. E., Seligman, D., & Kable, J. W. (2012) Preference reversals in decision making under risk are accompanied by changes in attention to different attributes. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6:109. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00109.

Kim, J. S.-C., Vossel, G., Gamer, M. (2013). Effects of emotional context on memory for details: The role of attention. PLoS ONE 8(10): e77405. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077405.

Kim, P., Arizpe, J., Rosen, B. H., Razdan, V., Haring, C. T., Jenkins, S. E., Deveney, C. M., Brotman, M. A., Blair, R. J. R., Pine, D. S., Baker, C. I., & Leibenluft, E. (2013). Impaired fixation to eyes during facial emotion labelling in children with bipolar disorder or severe mood dysregulation. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 38, 407-416.

Kim, Y.-S., Radach, R., & Vorstius, C. (2012). Eye movements and parafoveal processing during reading in Korean. Reading and Writing, 25, 1053-1078.

Kimble, M. O., Fleming, K., Bandy, C., Kim, J., & Zambetti, A. (2010). Eye tracking and visual attention to threating stimuli in veterans of the Iraq war. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 293-299.

Kimmel, D. L., Mammo, D., & Newsome, W. T. (2012). Tracking the eye non-invasively: Simultaneous comparison of the scleral search coil and optical tracking techniques in the macaque monkey. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6:49. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00049.

Kinder, A., Rolfs, M., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Sequence learning at optimal stimulus-response mapping: Evidence from a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 203-209.

Kirk, H. E., Hocking, D. R., Riby, D. M., & Cornish, K. M. (2013). Linking social behaviour and anxiety to attention to emotional faces in Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 4608-4616.

Kirkby, J. A., Blythe, H. I., Drieghe, D., Benson, V., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Investigating eye movement acquisition and analysis technologies as a causal factor in differential prevalence of crossed and uncrossed fixation disparity during reading and dot scanning. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 664-678.

Kis, A. C., Singh, V. W. A., & Niemeier, M. (2009). Short- and long-term plasticity of eye position information: Examining perceptual, attentional, and motor influences on perisaccadic perception. Journal of Vision, 9(6):11, 1-20, http://journalofvision.org/9/6/11/, doi:10.1167/9.6.11.

Kita, Y., Gunji, A., Inoue, Y., Goto, T., Sakihara, K., Kaga, M., Inagaki, M., & Hosokawa, T. (2011). Self-face recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain and Development, 33, 494-503.

Kita. Y., Gunji, A., Sakihara, K., Inagaki, M., Kaga, M., Nakagawa, E., & Hosokawa, T. (2010). Scanning strategies do not modulate face identification: Eye-tracking and near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS ONE 5(6): e11050. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011050.

Klein, C. H., & Berg, P. (2001). Four-week test-retest stability of individual differences in the saccadic CNV, two saccadic task parameters, and selected neuropsychological tests. Psychophysiology, 38, 704-711.

Klein, J. T., & Platt, M. L. (2013). Social information signaling by neurons in primate striatum. Current Biology, 23, 691-696.

Klein, R. M., & MacInnes, W. J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10, 346-352.

Klein, R. M., Christie, J., & Morris, E. P. (2005). Vector averaging of inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 295-300.

Kliegl, R., & Engbert, R. (2005). Fixation durations before word skipping in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 132-138.

Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. (2004). Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 262-284.

Kliegl, R., Hohenstein, S., Yan, M., & McDonald, S. A. (2013). How preview space/time translates into preview cost/benefit for fixation durations during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 581-600.

Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 12-35.

Kliegl, R., Risse, S., & Laubrock, J. (2007). Preview benefit and parafoveal-on-foveal effects from word n + 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 1250-1255.

Kliegl, R., Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Engbert, R. (2009). Microsaccadic modulation of response times in spatial attention tasks. Psychological Research, 73, 136-146.

Klingenhoefer, S., & Bremmer, F. (2009). Perisaccadic localization of auditory stimuli. Experimental Brain Research, 198, 411-423.

Klingenhoefer, S., & Bremmer, F. (2011). Saccadic suppression of displacement in face of saccade adaptation. Vision Research, 51, 881-889.

Klink, P. C., Oleksiak, A., Lankheet, M. J. M., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2012). Intermittent stimulus presentation stabilizes neuronal responses in macaque area MT. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 2101-2114.

Klink, P. C., van Ee, R., Nijs, M. M., Brouwer, G. J., Noest, A. J., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2008). Early interactions between neuronal adaptation and voluntary control determine perceptual choices in bistable vision. Journal of Vision, 8(5):16, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/8/5/16/, doi:10.1167/8.5.16.

Kloft, L., Kischkel, E., Kathmann, N., & Reuter, B. (2011). Evidence for a deficit in volitional action generation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology, 48, 755-761.

Kloft, L., Reuter, B., Riesel, A., & Kathmann, N. (2013). Impaired volitional saccade control: first evidence for a new candidate endophenotype in obsessive-compulsive disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, 215-222.

Kloft, L., Reuter, B., Viswanathan, J., Kathmann, N., & Barton, J. J. (2012). Response selection in prosaccades, antisaccades, and other volitional saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 345-353.

Kloke, W. B., Jaschinski, W., & Jainta, S. (2009). Microsaccades under monocular viewing conditions. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(1):2, 1-7.

Kluge, C., Bauer, M., Leff, A. P., Heinze, H.-J., Dolan, R. J., & Driver, J. (2011). Plasticity of human auditory-evoked fields induced by shock conditioning and contingency reversal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 12545-12550.

Klöppel, S., Draganski, B., Golding, C. V., Chu, C., Nagy, Z., Cook, P. A., Hicks, S. L., Kennard, C., Alexander, D. C., Parker, G. J. M., Tabrizi, S. J., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (2008). White matter connections reflect changes in voluntary-guided saccades in pre-symptomatic Huntington's disease. Brain, 131, 196-204.

Knapen, T. H. J., & van Ee, R. (2006). Slant perception, and its voluntary control, do not govern the slant aftereffect: Multiple slant signals adapt independently. Vision Research, 46, 3381-3392.

Knapen, T., Brascamp, J., Adams, W. J., & Graf, E. W. (2009). The spatial scale of perceptual memory in ambiguous figure perception. Journal of Vision, 9(13):16, 1-12, http://journalofvision.org/9/13/16/, doi:10.1167/9.13.16.

Knapen, T., Rolfs, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). The reference frame of the motion aftereffect is retinotopic. Journal of Vision, 9(5):16, 1-6, http://journalofvision.org/9/5/16/, doi:10.1167/9.5.16.

Knapen, T., Rolfs, M., Wexler, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2010). The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect. Journal of Vision, 10(1):8, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/10/1/8/, doi:10.1167/10.1.8.

Knapp, C. M., Gottlob, I., McLean, R. J., & Proudlock, F. A. (2008). Horizontal and vertical look and stare optokinetic nystagmus symmetry in healthy adult volunteers. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 49, 581-588.

Knapp, C. M., Gottlob, I., McLean, R. J., Rafelt, S., & Proudlock, F. A. (2009). Effect of distance upon horizontal and vertical look and stare OKN. Journal of Vision, 9(12):23, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/9/12/23/, doi:10.1167/9.12.23.

Knapp, C. M., Gottlob, I., McLean, R. J., Rajabally, Y. A., Abbott, R. J., Rafelt, S., & Proudlock, F. A. (2009). Vertical optokinetic nystagmus in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 24, 1533-1538.

Knapp, C.M., Proudlock, F.A., & Gottlob, I. (2013). OKN asymmetry in human subjects: a literature review. Strabismus, 21, 37-49.

Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M. (2006). The coordinated interplay of scene, utterance, and world knowledge: evidence from eye tracking. Cognitive Science, 30, 481-529.

Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M. W. (2007). The influence of recent scene events on spoken comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 519-543.

Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M. W. (2009). Constituent order and semantic parallelism in on-line comprehension: eye-tracking evidence from German. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 2338 - 2371.

Knoeferle, P., & Kreysa H. (2012). Can speaker gaze modulate syntactic structuring and thematic role assignment during spoken sentence comprehension? Frontiers in Psychology, 3:538. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00538.

Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M., Pickering, M., & Scheepers, C. (2005). The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role-assignment: Evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 95, 95-127.

Knöll, J., Binda, P., Morrone, M. C., & Bremmer, F. (2011). Spatiotemporal profile of peri-saccadic contrast sensitivity. Journal of Vision, 11(14):15, 1–12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/15, doi:10.1167/11.14.15.

Knöll, J., Morrone, M. C. M., & Bremmer, F. (2013). Spatio-temporal topography of saccadic overestimation of time. Vision Research, 83, 56-65.

Kobayashi, M. & Sugiyama, a. (2012). Decreased ratio of downward to horizontal smooth pursuit eye movement velocity in a patient with Chiari I Malformation: Application in early detection of vestibulocerebellar malfunction. Internal Medicine, 51, 2025-2029.

Koehn, J. D., Roy, E., & Barton, J. J. S. (2008). The 'diagonal effect': a systematic error in oblique antisaccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 587-597.

Koenig, S., & Lachnit, H. (2011). Curved saccade trajectories reveal conflicting predictions in associative learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1164-1177.

Koh, S., Yoon, N., Yoon, S.-O., & Pollatsek, A, (2013). Word frequency and root-morpheme frequency effects on processing of Korean particle-suffixed words. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 64-72.

Kohler, P. J., Caplovitz, G. P., & Tse, P. U. (2009). The whole moves less than the spin of its parts. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 675-679.

Kohler, P. J., Caplovitz, G. P., Hsieh, P. -J., Sun, J., & Tse, P. U. (2010). Motion fading is driven by perceived, not actual angular velocity. Vision Research, 50, 1086-1094.

Koivisto, M., Hyönä, J., & Revonsuo, A. (2004). The effects of eye movements, spatial attention and stimulus features on inattentional blindness. Vision Research, 44, 3211-3221.

Kok, E. M., de Bruin, A. B. H., Robben, S. G. F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2012) Looking in the same manner but seeing it differently: Bottom-up and expertise effects in radiology. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 854-862.

Kolarik, A. J., Margrain, T. H., & Freeman, T. C. A. (2010). Precision and accuracy of ocular following: influence of age and type of eye movement. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 271-282.

Kollmorgen, S., Nortmann, N., Schröder, S., & König, P. (2010). Influence of low-level stimulus features, task dependent factors, and spatial biases on overt visual attention. PLoS Computional Biology, 6(5): e1000791. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000791.

Komogortsev, O. V., & Karpov, A. (2013). Automated classification and scoring of smooth pursuit eye movements in the presence of fixations and saccades. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 203-215.

Koornneef, W., & Sanders, T. J. M. (2013). Establishing coherence relations in discourse: The influence of implicit causality and connectives on pronoun resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1169-1206.

Kootstra, G., de Boer, B., & Schomaker, L. R. B. (2011). Predicting eye fixations on complex visual stimuli using local symmetry. Cognitive Computation, 3, 223-240.

Korvorst, M., Roelofs, A., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2006). Incrementality in naming and reading complex numerals: Evidence from eyetracking. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 296-311.

Kosilo, M., Wuerger, S.M., Craddock, M., Jennings, B.J., Hunt, A.R., & Martinovic, J. (2013). Low-level and high-level modulations of fixational saccades and high frequency oscillatory brain activity in a visual object classification task. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:948. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00948.

Kotani, K., Yamaguchi, Y., Asao, T., & Horii, K. (2010). Design of eye-typing interface using saccadic latency of eye movement. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 26, 361-376.

Kotowicz, A., Rutishauser, U., & Koch, C. (2010). Time course of target recognition in visual search. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4:31. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00031.

Kouider, S., Berthet, V., & Faivre, N. (2011). Preference is biased by crowded facial expressions. Psychological Science, 22, 184-189.

Kourkoulou, A., Kuhn, G., Findlay, J.M., & Leekam, S. R. (2013). Eye movement difficulties in autism spectrum disorder: implications for implicit contextual learning. Autism Research, 6, 177-189.

Kourtzi, Z., & Huberle, E. (2005). Spatiotemporal characteristics of form analysis in the human visual cortex revealed by rapid event-related fMRI adaptation. NeuroImage, 28, 440-452.

Kourtzi, Z., Betts, L. R., Sarkheil, P., & Welchman, A. E. (2005). Distributed neural plasticity for shape learning in the human visual cortex. PLoS Biology, 3, e204.

Koval, M. J., Lomber, S. G., & Everling, S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex deactivation in macaques alters activity in the superior colliculus and impairs voluntary control of saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8659-8668.

Krab, L. C., de Goede-Bolder, A., Aarsen, F. K., Moll, H. A., De Zeeuw, C. I., Elgersma, Y., & van der Geest, J. N. (2011). Motor learning in children with Neurofibromatosis Type I. The Cerebellum, 10, 14-21.

Kramer, A. F., Boot, W. R., McCarley, J. S., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, A., & Scialfa, C. T. (2006). Aging, memory and visual search. Acta Psychologica, 122, 288-304.

Kramer, A. F., Cassavaugh, N., Irwin, D. E., Peterson, M. S., & Hahn, S. (2001). Influence of single and multiple onset distractors on visual search for singleton targets. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 952-968.

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Irwin, D. E., & Theeuwes, J. (1999). Attentional capture and aging: Implications for visual search performance and oculomotor control. Psychology & Aging, 14, 135-154.

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Irwin, D. E., & Theeuwes, J. (2000). Age difference in the control of looking behavior: Do you know where your eyes have been? Psychological Science, 11, 210-217.

Kramer, A. F., de Sather, J. C., & Cassavaugh, N. D. (2005). Development of attentional and oculomotor control. Developmental Psychology, 41, 760-772.

Kreiner, H., Garrod, S., & Sturt, P. (2013). Number agreement in sentence comprehension: The relationship between grammatical and conceptual factors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 829-874.

Krekelberg, B., Vatakis, A., & Kourtzi, Z. (2005). Implied motion from form in the human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 4373-4386.

Kret, M.E., Roelofs, K., Stekelenburg, J.J., & de Gelder, B. (2013). Emotional signals from faces, bodies and scenes influence observers' face expressions, fixations and pupil-size. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:810. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00810.

Kret, M.E., Stekelenburg. J.J., Roelofs, K., & de Gelder, B. (2013). Perception of face and body expressions using electromyography, pupillometry and gaze measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:28. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00028.

Kretzschmar, F., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2009). Parafoveal versus foveal N400s dissociate spreading activation from contextual fit. NeuroReport, 20, 1613-1618.

Kretzschmar, F., Pleimling, D., Hosemann, J., Füssel, S., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky. I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2013). Subjective impressions do not mirror online reading effort: Concurrent EEG-eyetracking evidence from the reading of books and digital media. PLoS ONE 8(2): e56178. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056178.

Kriz, S., &, Hegarty, M. (2007). Top-down and bottom-up influences on learning from animations. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 65, 911-930.

Kruger, H. M., & Hunt, A. R. (2013). Inhibition of return across eye and object movements: The role of prediction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 735-744.

Krügel, A., & Engbert, R. (2010). On the launch-site effect for skipped words during reading. Vision Research, 50, 1532-1539.

Krügel, A., Vitu, F., & Engbert, R. (2012). Fixation positions after skipping saccades: A single space makes a large difference. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1556-1561.

Kuchinsky, S. E., Bock, K., & Irwin, D. E. (2011). Reversing the hands of time: Changing the mapping from seeing to saying. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 748-756.

Kuhn, G., & Findlay, J. M. (2010). Misdirection, attention and awareness: Inattentional blindness reveals temporal relationship between eye movements and visual awareness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 136-146.

Kuhn, G., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Look away! Eyes and arrows engage oculomotor responses automatically? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 314-327.

Kuhn, G., & Tipples, J. (2011). Increased gaze following for fearful faces. It depends on what you’re looking for! Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 89-95.

Kuhn, G., Benson, V., Fletcher-Watson, S., Kovshoff, H., McCormick, C. A., Kirkby, J., & Leekam, S. R. (2010). Eye movements affirm: automatic overt gaze and arrow cueing for typical adults and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 155-165.

Kuhn, G., Kourkoulou, A., & Leekam, S. R. (2010). How magic changes our expectations about autism. Psychological Science, 21, 1487-1493.

Kuhn, G., Tatler, B. W., & Cole, G. (2009). You look where I look! Effect of gaze cues on overt and covert attention in misdirection. Visual Cognition, 17, 925-944.

Kuhn, G., Tewson, L., Morpurgo, L., Freebody, S. F., Musil, A. S., & Leekam, S. R. (2011). Developmental changes in the control of saccadic eye movements in response to directional eye gaze and arrows. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1919-1929.

Kumar, A., Gottlob, I., Mclean, R. J., Thomas, S., Thomas, M. G., & Proudlock, F. A. (2011). Clinical and oculomotor characteristics of albinism compared to FRMD7 associated infantile nystagmus. Investigative Ophthalmological and Visual Science, 52, 2306 - 2313.

Kumar, A., Sarvananthan, N., Proudlock, F., Thomas, M., Roberts, E., & Gottlob, I. (2009). Asperger Syndrome associated with idiopathic infantile nystagmus: A report of 2 cases. Strabismus, 17, 63-65.

Kumar, A., Thomas, S., McLean, R., Proudlock, F. A., Roberts, E., Boggild, M., & Gottlob, I. (2009). Treatment of Acquired Periodic Alternating Nystagmus With Memantine: A Case Report. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 32, 109-110.

Kuo, F.-Y., Hsu, C.-W., & Day, R.-F. (2009). An exploratory study of cognitive effort involved in decision under framing-an application of the eye-tracking technology. Decision Support Systems, 48, 81-91.

Kuperman, V., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2011). Effects of individual differences in verbal skills on eye-movement patterns during sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 42-73.

Kuperman, V., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2013). Reassessing word frequency as a determinant of word recognition for skilled and unskilled readers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 802-823.

Kuperman, V., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2008). Morphological dynamics in compound processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 1089-1132.

Kuperman, V., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2010). Processing trade-offs in the reading of Dutch derived words. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 83-97.

Kuperman, V., Dambacher, M., Nuthmann, A., & Kliegl, R. (2010). The effect of word position on eye movements in sentence and paragraph reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1838-1857.

Kuperman, V., Schreuder, R., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Reading polymorphemic Dutch compounds: Toward a multiple route model of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 876-895.

Kushnir, T., Hendler, T., Malach, R., & Grill-Spector, K. (2000). The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognitionperformance in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 837-843.

Kwon, M. Y., Nandy, A. S., & Tjan, B. S. (2013). Rapid and persistent adaptability of human oculomotor control in response to simulated central vision loss. Current Biology, 23, 1663-1669.

Kwon, M., Ramachandra, C., Satgunam, P., Mel, B. W., Peli, E., & Tjan, B. S. (2012). Contour enhancement benefits older adults with simulated central field loss. Optometry & Vision Science, 89, 1374-84. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3182678e52.

Kwon, N., Lee, Y., Gordon, P., Kluender, R., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic factors affecting subject/object asymmetry: An eye-tracking study of pre-nominal relative clauses in Korean. Language, 86, 546-582.

Königs, K., & Bremmer, F. (2010). Localization of visual and auditory stimuli during smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision, 10(8):8, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/8, doi:10.1167/10.8.8.

Königs, K., Knöll, J., & Bremmer, F. (2007). Localisation of auditory targets during optokinetic nystagmus. Perception, 36, 1507-1512.

Körner, C. (2004). Sequential processing in comprehension of hierarchical graphs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 467-480.

Körner, C. (2011). Eye movements reveal distinct search and reasoning processes in comprehension of complex graphs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 893-905.

Körner, C., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2004). Eye movements in a simple spatial reasoning task. Perception, 33, 485-494.

Körner, C., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2007). Finding a new target in an old display: Evidence for a memory recency effect in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 846-851.

Körner, C., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2008). Memory processes in multiple-target visual search. Psychological Research, 72, 99-105.

Back To Top

L

Lagrou, E., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2013). Interlingual lexical competition in a spoken sentence context: Evidence from the visual world paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 963-972.

Laidlaw, K. E. W., & Kingstone, A. (2010). The time course of vertical, horizontal and oblique saccade trajectories: Evidence for greater distractor interference during vertical saccades. Vision Research, 50, 829-837.

Laidlaw, K. E. W., Risko, E. F., & Kingstone, A. (2012). A new look at social attention: Orienting to the eyes is not (entirely) under volitional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1132-1143.

Lam, S. Y., Chau, A. W.-L., & Wong, T. J. (2007). Thumbnails as online product displays: How consumers process them. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21, 36-59.

Lambert, E., Alamargot, D., Larocque, D., & Caporossi, G. (2011). Dynamics of the spelling process during a copy task: Effects of regularity and frequency. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 141-150.

Lamers, M. J. M., & Roelofs, A. (2011). Attention and gaze shifting in dual-task and go/no-go performance with vocal responding. Acta Psychologica, 137, 261-268.

Lamontagne, A., & Fung, J. (2009). Gaze and postural reorientation in the control of locomotor steering after stroke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 23, 256-266.

Lamontagne, A., Paquette, C., & Fung, J. (2007). Stroke affects the coordination of gaze and posture during preplanned turns while walking. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 21, 62-67.

Lang, A., Vernet, M., Yang, Q., Orssaud, C., Londero, A., & Kapoula, Z. (2013). Differential auditory-oculomotor interactions in patients with right vs. left sided subjective tinnitus: a saccade study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:47. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00047.

Lange, E. B., & Engbert, R. (2013). Differentiating between verbal and spatial encoding using eye-movement recordings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1840-1857.

Lange, E. B., Starzynski, C., & Engbert, R. (2012). Capture of the gaze does not capture the mind. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1168-1182.

Lange, W.-G., Heuer, K., Langner, O., Keijsers, G. P. J., Becker, E. S., & Rinck, M. (2011). Face value: Eye movements and the evaluation of facial crowds in social anxiety. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 355-363.

Lansing C. R., & McConkie, G. W. (2003). Word identification and eye fixation locations in visual and visual-plus-auditory presentations of spoken sentences. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 536-552.

Lao, J., Vizioli, L., & Caldara, R. (2013). Culture modulates the temporal dynamics of global/local processing. Culture and Brain, 1. 158-174.

Lappe, M., Kuhlmann, S., Oerke, B., & Kaiser, M. (2006). The fate of object features during perisaccadic mislocalization. Journal of Vision, 6(11), 1282-1293, http://journalofvision.org/6/11/11/, doi:10.1167/6.11.11.

Lappe-Osthege, M., Talamo, S., Helmchen, C., & Sprenger, A. (2010). Overestimation of saccadic peak velocity recorded by electro-oculography compared to video-oculography and scleral search coil. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121, 1786-1787.

Laretzaki, G., Plainis, S., Vrettos, I., Chrisoulakis, A., Pallikaris, I., & Bitsios, P. (2011). Threat and trait anxiety affect gaze fixation stability. Biological Psychology, 86, 330-336.

Laubrock, J., Cajar, A., & Engbert, R. (2013). Control of fixation duration during scene viewing by interaction of foveal and peripheral processing. Journal of Vision, 13(12):11, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/12/11, doi:10.1167/13.12.11.

Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention. Vision Research, 45, 721-730.

Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Fixational eye movements predict the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent motion. Journal of Vision, 8(14):13, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/13/, doi:10.1167/8.14.13.

Laubrock, J., Kliegl, R., Rolfs, M., & Engbert, R. (2010). When do microsaccades follow spatial attention? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 683-694.

Lavergne, L., Vergilino-Perez, D., Lemoine, C., Collins, T., & Doré-Mazars, K. (2011). Exploring and targeting saccades dissociated by saccadic adaptation. Brain Research, 1415, 47-55.

Lavin, C., San_martín, R., & Rosales_jubal, E. (2013). Pupil dilation signals uncertainty and surprise in a learning gambling task. Frontiers in Behavoral Neuroscience, 7:218. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00218.

Le Dantec, C. C., & Seitz, A. R. (2012). High resolution, high capacity, spatial specificity in perceptual learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00222.

Le Dantec, C. C., Melton, E. E., & Seitz, A. R. (2012). A triple dissociation between learning of target, distractors, and spatial contexts. Journal of Vision, 12(2):5, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/2/5, doi:10.1167/12.2.5.

Le, A., & Niemeier, M. (2013). A right hemisphere dominance for bimanual grasps. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 263-273.

Le, A., & Niemeier, M. (2013). Left visual field preference for a bimanual grasping task with ecologically valid object sizes. Experimental Brain Research, 230, 187-196.

LeVasseur, A. L., Flanagan, J. R., Riopelle, R. J., & Munoz, D. P. (2001). Control of volitional and reflexive saccades in Tourette's syndrome. Brain, 124, 2045-2058.

Leavitt, M.L., Pieper, F., Sachs, A., Joober, R., & Martinez-Trujillo, J.C. (2013). Structure of spike count correlations reveals functional interactions between neurons in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex area 8a of behaving primates. PLoS ONE 8(4): e61503. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061503.

Ledberg, A., Montagnini, A., Coppola, R., Bressler, S. L. (2012). Reduced variability of ongoing and evoked cortical activity leads to improved behavioral performance. PLoS ONE 7(8): e43166. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043166.

Leder, H., Forster, M., & Gerger, G. (2011). The glasses stereotype revisited: Effects of eyeglasses on perception, recognition, and impression of faces. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70, 211-222.

Ledoux, K., Gordon, P. C., Camblin, C. C., & Swaab, T. Y. (2007). Coreference and lexical repetition: Neural mechanisms of discourse integration. Memory & Cognition, 35, 801-815.

Lee, A.K.C., Rajaram, S., Xia, J., Bharadwaj, H., Larson, E., Hämäläinen, M.S., & Shinn-Cunningham, B.G. (2013). Auditory selective attention reveals preparatory activity in different cortical regions for selection based on source location and source pitch. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6:190. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00190.

Lee, C.-L., Middleton, E., Mirman, D., Kalenine, S., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2013) Incidental and context-responsive activation of structure- and function-based action features during object identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 257-270.

Lee, E.-J., Kwon, G., Lee, A., Ghajar, J., & Suh, M. (2011). Individual differences in working memory capacity determine the effects of oculomotor task load on concurrent word recall performance. Brain Research, 1399, 59-65.

Lee, H., Abegg, M., Rodriguez, A., Koehn, J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2010). Why do humans make antisaccade errors? Experimental Brain Research, 201, 65-73.

Lee, K.-M., & Ahn, K.-H. (2013). The frontal eye fields limit the capacity of visual short-term memory in rhesus monkeys. PLoS ONE 8(3): e59606. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059606.

Lee, K.-M., & Keller, E. L. (2008). Neural activity in the frontal eye fields modulated by the number of alternatives in target choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 2242-2251.

Lee, K.-M., Ahn, K.-H., & Keller, E. L. (2012). Saccade generation by the frontal eye fields in rhesus monkeys is separable from visual detection and bottom-up attention shift. PLoS ONE 7(6): e39886. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039886.

Lee, R. J., & Smithson, H. E. (2012). Context-dependent judgments of color that might allow color constancy in scenes with multiple regions of illumination. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 29, A247-57. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A247.

Lee, Y., Lee, H., & Gordon, P. C. (2007). Linguistic complexity and information structure in Korean: Evidence from eye-tracking during reading. Cognition, 104, 495-534.

Lee, Y., Nam, K., & Gordon, P. C. (2009). Processing of the Korean Eojoel ambiguity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38, 345-362.

Leek, E. C., Patterson, C., Paul, M. A., Rafal, R., & Cristino, F. (2012). Eye movements during object recognition in visual agnosia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2142-2153.

Leff, A. P., Scott, S. K., Crewes, H., Hodgson, T. L., Cowey, A., Howard, D., & Wise, R. J. S. (2000). Impaired reading in patients with right hemianopia. Annals of Neurology, 47, 171-178.

Lehtimäki, T. M., & Reilly, R. G. (2005). Improving eye movement control in young readers. The Artificial Intelligence Review, 24, 477-488.

Leinenger, M., & Rayner, K. (2013). Eye movements while reading biased homographs: Effects of prior encounter and biasing context on reducing the subordinate bias effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 665-681.

Lemay, M., & Stelmach, G. E. (2005). Multiple frames of reference for pointing to a remembered target. Experimental Brain Research, 164, 301-310.

Lenzenweger, M. F., & O'Driscoll, G. A. (2006). Smooth pursuit eye movement and schizotypy in the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 779-786.

Lenzenweger, M. F., McLachlan, G., & Rubin, D. B. (2007). Resolving the latent structure of schizophrenia endophenotypes using expectation-maximization-based finite mixture modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 16-29.

Lenzner, A., Schnotz, W., & Müller, A. (2013). The role of decorative pictures in learning. Instructional Science, 41, 811-831.

Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2011). The role of magnocellular signals in oculomotor attentional capture. Journal of Vision, 11(13):11, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/13/11, doi:10.1167/11.13.11.

Leonards, U., & Mohr, C. (2009). Schizotypal personality traits influence idiosyncratic initiation of saccadic face exploration. Vision Research, 49, 2404-2413.

Leonards, U., & Scott-Samuel, N. E. (2005). Idiosyncratic initiation of saccadic face exploration in humans. Vision Research, 45, 2677-2684.

Leonards, U., Stone, S., & Mohr, C. (2013). Line bisection by eye and by hand reveal opposite biases. Experimental Brain Research, 228, 513-525.

Leopold, D. A., O'Toole, A., J., Vetter, T., & Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 89-94.

Leopold, D. A., Plettenberg, H. K., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual processing in the Ketamine-anesthetized monkey: Optokinetic and blood oxygenation level-dependent responses. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 359-372.

Lester, B. D., & Dassonville, P. (2011). Attentional control settings modulate susceptibility to the induced Roelofs effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1398-1406.

Lester, B. D., & Dassonville, P. (2013). Shifts of visuospatial attention do not cause the spatial distortions of the Roelofs effect. Journal of Vision, 13(12):4, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/12/4, doi:10.1167/13.12.4.

Lestou, V., Pollick, F. E., & Kourtzi, Z. (2008). Neural substrates for action understanding at different description levels in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 324-341.

Leung, H.-C., & Cai, W. (2007). Common and differential ventrolateral prefrontal activity during inhibition of hand and eye movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 9893-9900.

Levy, J., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Monsters are people too. Biology Letters, 9, 20120850.

Levy, R. P., & Keller, F. (2013). Expectation and locality effects in German verb-final structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 199-222.

Levy, R., Bicknell, K., Slattery, T., & Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movement evidence that readers maintain and act on uncertainty about past linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 21086-21090.

Lewis, M. R., & Mensink, M. C. (2012). Prereading Questions and Online Text Comprehension. Discourse Processes, 49, 367-390.

Lewis, R. L., Shvartsman, M., & Singh, S. (2013). The adaptive nature of eye movements in linguistic tasks: how payoff and architecture shape speed-accuracy trade-offs. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5, 581-610.

Leyland, L.-A., Kirkby, J. A., Juhasz, B. J., Pollatsek, A., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). The influence of word shading and word length on eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 471-486.

Li, C.-S. R., & Lin, S.-C. (2002). A perceptual level mechanism of the inhibition of return in oculomotor planning. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 269-276.

Li, C.-S. R., & Lin, S.-C. (2002). Inhibition of return in temporal order saccades. Vision Research, 42, 2089-2093.

Li, C.-S. R., Chang, H.-L., & Lin, S.-C. (2003). Inhibition of return in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Experimental Brain Research, 149, 125-130.

Li, C.-S. R., Chen, M.-C., Yang, Y.-Y., Chang, H.-L., Liu, C.-Y., Shen, S., & Chen, C. Y. (2000). Perceptual alternation in obsessive compulsive disorder: Implications for a role of the cortico-striatal circuitry in mediating awareness. Behavioural Brain Research, 111, 61-69.

Li, H-C. O., Brenner, E., Cornelissen, F. W., & Kim, E. S. (2002). Systematic distortion of perceived 2D shape during smooth pursuit eye-movements. Vision Research, 42, 2569-2575.

Li, N., Wang, X., Wang, Y., Wang, L., Ying, M., Han, R., Liu, Y., & Zhao, K. (2011). Investigation of the gene mutations in two Chinese families with X-linked infantile nystagmus. Molecular Vision, 17, 461-468.

Li, X., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 945-949.

Li, X., Gu, J., Liu, P., & Rayner, K. (2013). The advantage of word-based processing in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 879-889.

Li, X., Liu, P., & Rayner, K. (2011). Eye movement guidance in Chinese reading: Is there a preferred viewing location? Vision Research, 51, 1146-1156.

Li, X., Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (2010). Where do we look when we count? The role of eye movements in enumeration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 409-426.

Li, X., Rayner, K., & Cave, K. R. (2009). On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 525-552.

Li, X., Zhao, W., & Pollatsek, A. (2012). Dividing lines at the word boundary position helps reading in Chinese. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 929-934.

Li, X.-H., Jing, J., Zou, X.-B., Huang, X., Jin, Y., Wang, Q.-X., Chen, X.-B., Yang, B.-R., & Yang, S.-Y. (2009). Picture perception in Chinese dyslexic children: an eye-movement study. Chinese Medical Journal, 122, 267-71.

Liang, J.-R., Moshel, S., Zivotofsky, A. Z., Caspi, A., Engbert, R., Kliegl, R., & Havlin, S. (2005). Scaling of horizontal and vertical fixational eye movements. Physical Review E, 71, 031909.

Liang, W.-K., & Juan, C.-H. (2012). Modulation of motor control in saccadic behaviors by TMS over the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 741-752.

Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25, 381-390.

Liesker, H., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2010). Eye-hand coupling is not the cause of manual return movements when searching. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 221-227.

Lin, I.-F., & Gorea, A. (2011). Location and identity memory of saccade targets. Vision Research, 51, 323-332.

Lindberg, T., & Näsänen, R. (2003). The effect of icon spacing and size on the speed of icon processing in the human visual system. Displays, 24, 111-120.

Lindsay, S., Scheepers, C., & Kamide, Y. (2013). To dash or to dawdle: Verb-associated speed of motion influences eye movements during spoken sentence comprehension. PLoS ONE 8(6): e67187. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067187.

Lingnau, A., Schwarzbach, J., & Vorberg, D. (2008). Adaptive strategies for reading with a forced retinal location. Journal of Vision, 8(5):6, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/8/5/6/, doi:10.1167/8.5.6.

Lingnau, A., Schwarzbach, J., & Vorberg, D. (2010). (Un-) Coupling gaze and attention outside central vision. Journal of Vision, 10(11):13, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/11/13, doi:10.1167/10.11.13.

Liu, .-Q., Zhou, Q.-X., & Xie, F. (2013). Effects of sleep deprivation on pilot’s cognitive behavior in flight simulation. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8025, 45-54.

Liu, C.-H., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., Tseng, P., & Juan, C.-H. (2012). Investigation of bistable perception with the "silhouette spinner": Sit still, spin the dancer with your will. Vision Research, 60, 34-39.

Liu, C.-L., Chiau, H.-Y., Tseng, P., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., Muggleton, N. G., & Juan, C. H. (2010). The antisaccade cost is modulated by contextual experience of location probability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 1438-1447.

Liu, C.-L., Tseng, P., Chiau, H.-Y., Liang, W.-K., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C. H. (2011). The location probability effects of saccade reaction times are modulated in the frontal eye fields but not in the supplementary eye field. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1416-1425.

Liu, D., Wang, Y., & Zhou, X. (2011). Lexical- and perceptual-based object effects in the two-rectangle cueing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 138, 397-404.

Liu, H., Ding, G., Zhao, W., Wang, H., Liu, K., & Shi, L. (2011). Variation of drivers' visual features in long-tunnel entrance section on expressway. Journal of Transportation Safety & Security, 3, 27-37.

Liu, P., & Li, X. (2013). Optimal viewing position effects in the processing of isolated Chinese words. Vision Research, 81, 45-57.

Liu, T., & Hou, Y. (2011). Global feature-based attention to orientation. Journal of Vision, 11(10):8, 1-8, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/10/8, doi:10.1167/11.10.8.

Liu, T., & Hou, Y. (2013). A hierarchy of attentional priority signals in human frontoparietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 16606-16616.

Liu, T., & Mance, I. (2011). Constant spread of feature-based attention across the visual field. Vision Research, 51, 26-33.

Liu, T., & Pleskac, T. J. (2011). Neural correlates of evidence accumulation in a perceptual decision task. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 2383-2398.

Liu, T., Hospadaruk, L., Zhu, D. C., & Gardner, J. L. (2011). Feature-specific attentional priority signals in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4484-4495.

Liu, T., Slotnick, S. D., Serences, J. T., & Yantis, S. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 1334-1343.

Liu, T.-C., Fan, M. H.-M., & Paas, F. (2014). Effects of digital dictionary format on incidental acquisition of spelling knowledge and cognitive load during second language learning: Click-on vs. key-in dictionaries. Computers & Education, 70, 9-20.

Liu, W. Liu, C. K., Zhuang, D. M., Liu, Z. Q., & Yuan, X. G. (2011). Comparison of expert and novice eye movement behaviors during landing flight. Advanced Materials Research, 383-390, 2556-2560.

Loeber, S., & Duka, T. (2009). Acute alcohol decreases performance of an instrumental response to avoid aversive consequences in social drinkers. Psychopharmacology, 205, 577-587.

Loeber, S., & Duka, T. (2009). Acute alcohol impairs conditioning of a behavioural reward-seeking response and inhibitory control processes-implications for addictive disorders. Addiction, 104, 2013-2022.

Loeber, S., & Duka, T. (2009). Extinction learning of stimulus reward contingencies: The acute effects of alcohol. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 102, 56-62.

London, S., Bishop, C. W., & Miller, L. M. (2012). Spatial attention modulates the precedence effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1371-1379.

Longman, C. S., Lavric, A., & Monsell, S. (2013). More attention to attention? An eye-tracking investigation of selection of perceptual attributes during a task switch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1142-1151.

Lord, S., Archibald, N., Mosimann, U., Burn, D., & Rochester, L. (2012). Dorsal rather than ventral visual pathways discriminate freezing status in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 18, 1094-1096.

Lorenceau, J. (2012). Cursive writing with smooth pursuit eye movements. Current Biology, 22, 1506-1509.

Lowder, M. W., & Gordon, P. C. (2012). The pistol that injured the cowboy: Difficulty with inanimate subject-verb integration is reduced by structural separation. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 819-832.

Lowder, M. W., & Gordon, P. C. (2013). It's hard to offend the college: Effects of sentence structure on figurative-language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 993-1011.

Lowder, M. W., Choi, W., & Gordon, P. C. (2013). Word recognition during reading: The interaction between lexical repetition and frequency. Memory & Cognition, 41, 738-751.

Lucke, S., Lachnit, H., Koenig, S., & Uengoer, M. (2013). The informational value of contexts affects context-dependent learning. Learning & Behavior, 41, 285-297.

Ludwig, C. J. H., & Davies, J. R. (2011). Estimating the growth of internal evidence guiding perceptual decisions. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 61-92.

Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Measuring saccade curvature: A curve-fitting approach. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 34, 618-624.

Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Stimulus-driven and goal-driven control over visual selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 902-912.

Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2003). Goal-driven modulation of oculomotor capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 1243-1251.

Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2003). Target similarity affects saccade curvature away from irrelevant onsets. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 60-69.

Ludwig, C. J. H., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2006). The relative contributions of luminance contrast and task demands on saccade target selection. Vision Research, 46, 2743-2748.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Butler, S. H., Rossit, S., Harvey, M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2009). Modelling contralesional movement slowing after unilateral brain damage. Neuroscience Letters, 452, 1-4.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Eckstein, M. P., & Beutter, B. R. (2007). Limited flexibility in the filter underlying saccadic targeting. Vision Research, 47, 280-288.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Farrell, S., Ellis, L. A., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2009). The mechanism underlying inhibition of saccadic return. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 180-202.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Farrell, S., Ellis, L. A., Hardwicke, T. E., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2012). Context-gated statistical learning and its role in visual-saccadic decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 150-169.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Gilchrist, I. D., & McSorley, E. (2004). The influence of spatial frequency and contrast on saccade latencies. Vision Research, 44, 2597-2604.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Gilchrist, I. D., & McSorley, E. (2005). The remote distractor effect in saccade programming: channel interactions and lateral inhibition. Vision Research, 45, 1177-1190.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Gilchrist, I. D., McSorley, E., & Baddeley, R. J. (2005). The temporal impulse response underlying saccadic decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 9907-9912.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Mildinhall, J. W., & Gilchrist, I.D. (2007). A population coding account for systematic variation in saccadic dead time. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 795-805.

Ludwig, C. J. H., Ranson, A., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2008). Oculomotor capture by transient events: A comparison of abrupt onsets, offsets, motion, and flicker. Journal of Vision, 8(14):11, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/11/, doi:10.1167/8.14.11.

Lugtigheid, A. J., Brenner, E., & Welchman, A. E. (2011). Speed judgments of three-dimensional motion incorporate extraretinal information. Journal of Vision, 11(13):1, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/13/1, doi:10.1167/11.13.1.

Lukander, K. (2006). A system for tracking gaze on handheld devices. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 660-666.

Lukavsky, J. (2013). Eye movements in repeated multiple object tracking. Journal of Vision, 13(7):9, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/7/9, doi:10.1167/13.7.9.

Luke, S. G., & Christianson, K. (2013). The influence of frequency across the time course of morphological processing: Evidence from the transposed-letter effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 781-799.

Luke, S. G., & Henderson, J. M. (2013). Oculomotor and cognitive control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from mindless reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1230-1242.

Luke, S. G., Nuthmann, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2013). Eye movement control in scene viewing and reading: Evidence from the stimulus onset delay paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 10-15.

Luke, S.G., Schmidt, J., & Henderson, J.M. (2013). Temporal oculomotor inhibition of return and spatial facilitation of return in a visual encoding task. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:400. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00400.

Lukos, J. R., Snider, J., Hernandez, M. E., Tunik, E., Hillyard, S., & Poizner, H. (2013). Parkinson’s disease patients show impaired corrective grasp control and eye-hand coupling when reaching to grasp virtual objects. Neuroscience, 254, 205-221.

Luo, G., Garaas, T., Pomplun, M., & Peli, E. (2010). Inconsistency between peri-saccadic mislocalization and compression: Evidence for separate "what" and "where" visual systems. Journal of Vision, 10(12):32, 1-8, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/12/32, doi:10.1167/10.12.32.

Luo, Y., Yan, M., & Zhou, X. (2013). Prosodic boundaries delay the processing of upcoming lexical information during silent sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 915-930.

Lykins, A. D., Meana, M., & Kambe, G. (2006). Detection of differential viewing patterns to erotic and non-erotic stimuli using eye-tracking methodology. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 569-575.

Lykins, A. D., Meana, M., & Strauss, G. P. (2008). Sex differences in visual attention to erotic and non-erotic stimuli. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 219-238.

Lünenburger, L., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (2003). Arm movement and gap as factors influencing the reaction time of the second saccade in a double-step task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2481-2491.

Back To Top

M

Ma-Wyatt, A., Stritzke, M., & Trommershäuser, J. (2010). Eye-hand coordination while pointing rapidly under risk. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 131-145.

MacKenzie, D. E., & Westwood, D. A. (2013). Observation patterns of dynamic occupational performance. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 80, 92-100.

MacKenzie, D. E., & Westwood, D. A. (2013). Occupational therapists and bbservation: What are you looking at? OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health, 33, 4-11.

MacKenzie, K. J., Dickson, R. A., & Watt, S. J. (2012). Vergence and accommodation to multiple-image-plane stereoscopic displays: "real world" responses with practical image-plane separations? Journal of Electronic Imaging, 21, 011002, http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.JEI.21.1.011002.

MacKenzie, K. J., Hoffman, D. M., & Watt, S. J. (2010). Accommodation to multiple focal plane displays: Implications for improving stereoscopic displays and for accommodation control. Journal of Vision, 10(8):22, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/22, doi:10.1167/10.8.22.

MacNamara, A., Schmidt, J., Zelinsky, G. J., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Electrocortical and ocular indices of attention to fearful and neutral faces presented under high and low working memory load. Biological Psychology, 91, 349-356.

Macedo, A. F., Crossland, M. D., & Rubin, G. S. (2008). The effect of retinal image slip on peripheral visual acuity. Journal of Vision, 8(14):16, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/16/, doi:10.1167/8.14.16.

Macedo, A. F., Crossland, M. D., & Rubin, G. S. (2011). Investigating unstable fixation in patients with macular disease. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 52, 1275-1280.

Machner, B., Klein, C., Sprenger, A., Baumbach, P., Pramstaller, P. P., Helmchen, C., & Heide, W. (2010). Eye movement disorders are different in Parkin-linked and idiopathic early-onset PD. Neurology, 75, 125-128.

Machner, B., Sprenger, A., Behrens, M. I., Ramirez, A., Brüggemann, N., Klein, C., & Helmchen, C. (2010). Eye movement disorders in ATP13A2 mutation carriers (PARK9). Movement Disorders, 25, 2687-2689.

Mackay, M., Cerf, M., & Koch, C. (2012). Evidence for two distinct mechanisms directing gaze in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 12(4):9, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/4/9, doi:10.1167/12.4.9.

Madelain, L., Herman, J. P., & Harwood, M. R. (2013). Saccade adaptation goes for the goal. Journal of Vision, 13(4):9, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/4/9, doi:10.1167/13.4.9.

Madsen, A. M., Larson, A. M., Loschky, L. C., & Rebello, N. S. (2012). Differences in visual attention between those who correctly and incorrectly answer physics problems. Physical Review Special Topics - Physics Education Research. 05/2012; 8(1):010122-1-13.

Madsen, A., Rouinfar, A., Larson, A. M., Loschky, L. C., & Rebello, N. S. (2013). Can short duration visual cues influence students' reasoning and eye movements in physics problems? Physical Review Special Topics - Physics Education Research, 9, 020104.

Maeda, F., Kanai, R., & Shimojo, S. (2004). Changing pitch induced visual motion illusion. Current Biology, 14, R990-R991.

Maffei, V., Macaluso, E., Indovina, I., Orban, G. A., & Lacquaniti, F. (2010). Processing of targets in smooth or apparent motion along the vertical in the human brain: an fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 360-370.

Magnuson, J. S., Dixon, J. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 1-24.

Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2008). Immediate effects of form-class constraints on spoken word recognition. Cognition, 108, 866-873.

Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Dahan, D. (2003). The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: Studies with artificial lexicons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 202-227.

Maier, A., Adams, G. K., Aura, C., & Leopold, D. A. (2010). Distinct superficial and deep laminar domains of activity in the visual cortex during rest and stimulation. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 4:31. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00031.

Maier, A., Aura, C. J., Leopold, D. A. (2011). Infragranular sources of sustained local field potential responses in macaque primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1971-1980.

Maij, F., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2009). Temporal information can influence spatial localization. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 490-495.

Maij, F., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2011). Peri-saccadic mislocalization is not influenced by the predictability of the saccade target location. Vision Research, 51, 154-159.

Maij, F., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2011). Temporal uncertainty separates flashes from their background during saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3708-3711.

Maij, F., Brenner, E., Li, H.-C. O., Cornelissen, F. W., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2010). The use of the saccade target as a visual reference when localizing flashes during saccades. Journal of Vision, 10(4):7, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/10/4/7/, doi:10.1167/10.4.7.

Maij, F., Matziridi, M., Smeets, J. B. J., & Brenner, E. (2012). Luminance contrast in the background makes flashes harder to detect during saccades. Vision Research, 60, 22-27.

Mak, W. M., & Sanders, T. J. M. (2013). The role of causality in discourse processing: Effects of expectation and coherence relations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1414-1437.

Makin, A. D. J., Ackerley, R., Wild, K., Poliakoff, E., Gowen, E., & El-Deredy, W. (2011). Coherent illusory contours reduce microsaccade frequency. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2798-2801.

Malcolm, G. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). The effects of target template specificity on visual search in real-world scenes: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Vision, 9(11):8, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/9/11/8/, doi:10.1167/9.11.8.

Malcolm, G. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2010). Combining top-down processes to guide eye movements during real-world scene search. Journal of Vision, 10(2):4, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/10/2/4/, doi:10.1167/10.2.4.

Malcolm, G. L., Lanyon, L. J., Fugard, A. J. B., & Barton, J. J. S. (2008). Scan patterns during the processing of facial expression versus identity: An exploration of task-driven and stimulus-driven effects. Journal of Vision, 8(8):2, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/8/8/2/, doi:10.1167/8.8.2.

Malhotra, P., Mannan, S., Driver, J., & Husain, M. (2004). Impaired spatial working memory: One component of the visual neglect syndrome? Cortex, 40, 667-676.

Malkinson, T., McKyton, A., & Zohary, E. Motion adaptation reveals that the motion vector is represented in multiple coordinate frames. Journal of Vision, 12(6):30, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/6/30, doi:10.1167/12.6.30.

Malpass, D., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). The time course of name retrieval during multiple-object naming: Evidence from extrafoveal-on-foveal effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 36, 523-537.

Malsert, J., Guyader, N., Chauvin, A., & Marendaz, C. (2012). Having to identify a target reduces antisaccade latencies in mixed saccadic paradigms: A top-down effect released by tonic prefrontal activation? Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 105-111.

Malsert, J., Guyader, N., Chauvin, A., Polosan, M., Poulet, E., Szekely, D., Bougerol, T., & Marendaz, C. (2012). Antisaccades as a follow-up tool in major depressive disorder therapies: A pilot study. Psychiatry Research, 200, 1051-1053.

Malsert, J., Guyader, N., Chauvin, A., Polosan, M., Szekely, D., Bougerol, T., & Marendaz, C. (2013). Saccadic performance and cortical excitability as trait-markers and state-markers in rapid cycling bipolar disorder: a two-case follow-up study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 3:112. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00112.

Mannan, S. K., Hodgson, T. L., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2008). Eye movements in visual search indicate impaired saliency processing in Parkinson's disease. Progress in Brain Research, 171, 559-562.

Mannan, S. K., Kennard, C., Potter, D., Pan, Y., & Soto, D. (2010). Early oculomotor capture by new onsets driven by the contents of working memory. Vision Research, 50, 1590-1597.

Mannan, S. K., Mort, D. J., Hodgson, T. L., Driver, J., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2005). Revisiting previously searched locations in visual neglect: Role of right parietal and frontal lesions in misjudging old locations as new. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 340-354.

Manohar, S.G., & Husain, M. (2013). Attention as foraging for information and value. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:711. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00711.

Mansour, J. K., Lindsay, R. C. L., Brewer, N., & Munhall, K. G. (2009). Characterizing visual behaviour in a lineup task. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1012-1026.

Marat, S., Phuoc, T. H., Granjon, L., Guyader, N., Pellerin, D., & Guérin-Dugué, A. (2009). Modelling spatio-temporal saliency to predict gaze direction for short videos. International Journal of Computer Vision, 82, 231-243.

Marat, S., Rahman, A., Pellerin, D., Guyader, N., & Houzet, D. (2013). Improving visual saliency by adding 'Face Feature Map' and 'Center Bias'. Cognitive Computation, 5, 63-75.

Marchitto, M., Di Stasi, L. L., & Cañas, J. J. (2012). Ocular movements under taskload manipulations: Influence of geometry on saccades in air traffic control simulated tasks. Human Factors and Ergonomics in Manufacturing & Service Industries, 22, 407-419.

Marelli, M., & Luzzatti, C. (2012). Frequency effects in the processing of Italian nominal compounds: Modulation of headedness and semantic transparency. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 644-664.

Marelli, M., Amenta, S., Morone, E. A., & Crepaldi, D. (2013). Meaning is in the beholder’s eye: Morpho-semantic effects in masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 534-541.

Marsh, P. J., Luckett, G., Russell, T., Coltheart, M., & Green, M. J. (2012). Effects of facial emotion recognition remediation on visual scanning of novel face stimuli. Schizophrenia Research, 141, 234-240.

Marshall, L., & Bays, P. M. (2013). Obligatory encoding of task-irrelevant features depletes working memory resources. Journal of Vision, 13(2):21, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/2/21, doi:10.1167/13.2.21.

Marshall, S. P. (2007). Identifying cognitive state from eye metrics. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 78, B165-175.

Marshall, S. P. (2009). What the eyes reveal: Measuring the cognitive workload of teams. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5620, 265-274.

Marsman, J.C., Renken, R., Haak, K.V., & Cornelissen, F.W. (2013). Linking cortical visual processing to viewing behavior using fMRI. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:109. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00109.

Martin, A. E., & McElree, B. (2008). A content-addressable pointer mechanism underlies comprehension of verb-phrase ellipsis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 879-906.

Martin, A. E., & McElree, B. (2011). Direct-access retrieval during sentence comprehension: Evidence from Sluicing. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 327-343.

Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., Tronconso, X. G., & Dyar, T. A. (2006). Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron, 49, 297-305.

Maruta, J., Heaton, K. J., Kryskow, E. M., Maule, A. L., & Ghajar, J. (2013). Dynamic visuomotor synchronization: Quantification of predictive timing. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 289-300.

Maruta, J., Suh, M., Niogi, S. N., Mukherjee, P., & Ghajar, J. (2010). Visual tracking synchronization as a metric for concussion screening. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 25, 293-305.

Marwan, N., Romano, M. C., Thiel, M., & Kurths, J. (2007). Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Physics Reports, 438, 237-329.

Masserang, K. M., & Pollatsek, A. (2012). Transposed letter effects in prefixed words: Implications for morphological decomposition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 476-495.

Mastropasqua, T., & Turatto, M. (2013). Perceptual grouping enhances visual plasticity. PLoS ONE 8(1): e53683. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053683.

Matheron, E., Yang, Q., Lê, T.-T., & Kapoula, Z. (2008). Effects of ocular dominance on the vertical vergence induced by a two-diopter vertical prism during standing. Neuroscience Letters, 444, 176-180.

Mathôt, S. & Theeuwes, J. (2012). It's all about the transient: Intra-saccadic onset stimuli do not capture attention. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(2):4, 1-12.

Mathôt, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Evidence for the predictive remapping of visual attention. Experimental Brain Research, 200, 117-122.

Mathôt, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Gradual remapping results in early retinotopic and late spatiotopic inhibition of return. Psychological Science, 21, 1793-1798.

Mathôt, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). A reinvestigation of the reference frame of the tilt-adaptation aftereffect. Scientific Reports, 3, Article number, 1152, doi:10.1038/srep01152.

Mathôt, S., van der Linden, L., Grainger, J., & Vitu, F. (2013). The pupillary light response reveals the focus of covert visual attention. PLoS ONE 8(10): e78168. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078168.

Matsuki, K., Chow, T., Hare, M., Elman, J. L., Scheepers, C., & McRae, K. (2011). Event-based plausibility immediately influences on-line language comprehension Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 913-934.

Matsukura, M., Brockmole, J. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Overt attentional prioritization of new objects and feature changes during real-world scene viewing. Visual Cognition, 17, 835-855.

Matsukura, M., Brockmole, J. R., Boot, W. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). Oculomotor capture during real-world scene viewing depends on cognitive load. Vision Research, 51, 546-552.

Matsumoto, H., Terao, Y., Furubayashi, T., Yugeta, A., Fukuda, H., Emoto, M., Hanajima, R., & Ugawa, Y. (2011). Small saccades restrict visual scanning area in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 26, 1619-1626.

Matsumoto, H., Terao, Y., Furubayashi, T., Yugeta, A., Fukuda, H., Emoto, M., Hanajima, R., & Ugawa, Y. (2012). Basal ganglia dysfunction reduces saccade amplitude during visual scanning in Parkinson's disease. Basal Ganglia, 2, 73-78.

Matsumoto, H., Terao, Y., Yugeta, A., Fukuda, H., Emoto, M., Furubayashi, T., Okano, T., Hanajima, R., & Ugawa, Y. (2011). Where do neurologists look when viewing brain CT images? An eye-tracking study involving stroke cases. PLoS ONE 6(12): e28928. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028928.

Matsumoto, M., & Takada, M. (2013). Distinct representations of cognitive and motivational signals in midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron, 79, 1011-1024.

Matthias, E., Bublak, P., Muller, H. J., Schneider, W. X., Krummenacher, J., & Finke, K. (2010). The influence of alertness on spatial and nonspatial components of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 36, 38-56.

Matziridi, M., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J.B.J. (2013). Is mislocalization during saccades related to the position of the saccade target within the image or to the gaze position at the end of the saccade? PLoS ONE 8(4): e62436. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0062436.

Maxcey-Richard, A. M., & Hollingworth, A. (2013). The strategic retention of task-relevant objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 760-772.

Maxfield, J. T., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2013). Searching through the hierarchy: How level of target categorization affects visual search. Visual Cognition, 20, 1153-1163.

Maynard, O. M., Munafo, M. R., & Leonards, U. (2013). Visual attention to health warnings on plain tobacco packaging in adolescent smokers and non-smokers. Addiction, 108, 413-419.

Mayr, U., Kuhns, D., & Rieter, M. (2013). Eye movements reveal dynamics of task control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 489-509.

Mazaheri, A., DiQuattro, N. E., Bengson, J., & Geng, J. J. (2011). Pre-stimulus activity predicts the winner of top-down vs. bottom-up attentional selection. PLoS ONE 6(2): e16243. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016243

Mazer, J. A., & Gallant, J. L. (2003). Goal-related activity in V4 during free viewing visual search: Evidence for a ventral stream visual salience map. Neuron, 40, 1241-1250.

McCabe, K. L., Melville, J. L., Rich, D., Strutt, P. A., Cooper, G., Loughland, C. M., Schall, U., & Campbell, L. E. (2013). Divergent patterns of social cognition performance in autism and 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1926-1934.

McCabe, K., Rich, D., Loughland, C. M., Schall, U., & Campbell, L. E. (2011). Visual scanpath abnormalities in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: Is this a face specific deficit? Psychiatry Research, 189, 292-298.

McCamy, M. B., Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S. L., Yang, Y., Troncoso, X. G., Baer, S. M., Crook, S. M., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2012). Microsaccadic efficacy and contribution to foveal and peripheral vision. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 9194-9204.

McCarley, J. S. (2009). Effects of speed-accuracy instructions on oculomotor scanning and target recognition in a simulated baggage X-ray screening task. Ergonomics, 52, 325-333.

McCarley, J. S., & Grant, C. (2008). State-trace analysis of the effects of a visual illusion on saccade amplitudes and perceptual judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1008-1014.

McCarley, J. S., Kramer, A. F., & Digirolamo, G. J. (2003). Differential effects of the Müller-Lyer illusion on reflexive and voluntary saccades. Journal of Vision. 3, 751-760. http://journalofvision.org/3/11/9/

McCarley, J. S., Kramer, A. F., & Peterson, M. S. (2002). Overt and covert object-based attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 751-758.

McCarley, J. S., Kramer, A. F., Boot, W. R., Peterson, M. S., Wang, R. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2006). Oculomotor behavior in visual search for multiple targets. Visual Cognition, 14, 685-703.

McCarley, J. S., Kramer, A. F., Wickens, C. D., Vidoni, E. D., & Boot, W. R. (2004). Visual skills in airport-security screening. Psychological Science, 15, 302-306.

McCarley, J. S., Wang, R., Kramer, A. F., Irwin, D. E., & Peterson, M. S. (2003). How much memory does oculomotor search have? Psychological Science, 14, 422-426.

McDonald, S. A. (2006). Effects of number-of-letters on eye movements during reading are independent from effects of spatial word length. Visual Cognition, 13, 89-98.

McDonald, S. A. (2006). Parafoveal preview benefit in reading is only obtained from the saccade goal. Vision Research, 46, 4416-4424.

McDonald, S. A., Spitsyna, G., Shillcock, R. C., Wise, R. J. S., & Leff, A. P. (2006). Patients with hemianopic alexia adopt an inefficient eye movement strategy when reading text. Brain, 129, 158-167.

McFarland, R., Roebuck, H., Yan, Y., Majolo, B., Li, W., & Guo, K.. (2013). Social interactions through the eyes of macaques and humans. PLoS ONE 8(2): e56437. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056437.

McKyton, A., & Zohary, E. (2007). Beyond retinotopic mapping: The spatial representation of objects in the human lateral occipital complex. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1164-1172.

McKyton, A., Pertzov, Y., & Zohary, E. (2009). Pattern matching is assessed in retinotopic coordinates. Journal of Vision, 9(13):19, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/9/13/19/, doi:10.1167/9.13.19.

McLean, R., Proudlock, F., Thomas, S., Degg, C., & Gottlob, I. (2007). Congenital nystagmus: Randomized, controlled, double-masked trial of memantine/gabapentin. Annals of Neurology, 61, 130-138.

McMullen, P. A., MacSween, L. E., & Collin, C. A. (2009). Behavioral effects of visual field location on processing motion- and luminance-defined form. Journal of Vision, 9(6):24, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/9/6/24/, doi:10.1167/9.6.24.

McMurray, B., Aslin, R. N., Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey, M. J., & Subik, D. (2008). Gradient sensitivity to within-category variation in words and syllables. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1609-1631.

McMurray, B., Clayards, M. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2008). Tracking the time course of phonetic cue integration during spoken word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 1064-1071.

McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 1-39.

McMurray, B., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Gradient effects of within-category phonetic variation on lexical access. Cognition, 86, B33-B42.

McMurray, B., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2009). Within-category VOT affects recovery from "lexical" garden-paths: Evidence against phoneme-level inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 65-91.

McQueen, J. M., & Huettig, F. (2012). Changing only the probability that spoken words will be distorted changes how they are recognized. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 131, 509-517.

McQueen, J. M., & Viebahn, M. C. (2007). Tracking recognition of spoken words by tracking looks to printed words. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 661-671.

McSorley, E., & Cruickshank, A. G. (2010). Evidence that indirect inhibition of saccade initiation improves saccade accuracy. i-Perception, 1, 73-82.

McSorley, E., & McCloy, R. (2009). Saccadic eye movements as an index of perceptual decision-making. Experimental Brain Research, 198, 513-520.

McSorley, E., & van Reekum, C. M. (2013). The time course of implicit affective picture processing: An eye movement study. Emotion, 13, 769-773.

McSorley, E., Cruickshank, A. G., & Inman, L. A. (2009). The development of the spatial extent of oculomotor inhibition. Brain Research, 1298, 92-98.

McSorley, E., Haggard, P., & Walker, R. (2004). Distractor modulation of saccade trajectories: Spatial separation and symmetry effects. Experimental Brain Research, 155, 320-333.

McSorley, E., Haggard, P., & Walker, R. (2005). Spatial and temporal aspects of oculomotor inhibition as revealed by saccade trajectories. Vision Research, 45, 2492-2499.

McSorley, E., Haggard, P., & Walker, R. (2006). Time-course of oculomotor inhibition revealed by saccade trajectory modulation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 1420-1424.

McSorley, E., Haggard, P., & Walker, R. (2009). The spatial and temporal shape of oculomotor inhibition. Vision Research, 49, 608-614.

McSorley, E., McCloy, R., & Lyne, C. (2012). The spatial impact of visual distractors on saccade latency. Vision Research, 60, 61-72.

Meadmore, K. L., Dror, I. E., Bucks, R. S., & Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Eye movements during visuospatial judgements. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 92-101.

Medendorp, W. P., Goltz, H. C., Crawford, J. D., & Vilis, T. (2005). Integration of target and effector information in human posterior parietal cortex for the planning of action. Journal of Neurophysiology, 93, 954-962.

Meek, B.P., Locheed, K., Lawrence-Dewar, J.M., Shelton, P., & Marotta, J.J. (2013). Posterior cortical atrophy: an investigation of scan paths generated during face matching tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:309. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00309.

Meeter, M., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2013). Visual priming through a boost of the target signal: Evidence from saccadic landing positions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1336-1341.

Megreya, A. M., Bindemann, M., Havard, C., & Burton, A. M. (2012). Identity-lineup location influences target selection: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 27, 167-178.

Megías, A., Maldonado, A., Catena, A., Di Stasi, L. L., Serrano, J., & Cándido, A. (2011). Modulation of attention and urgent decisions by affect-laden roadside advertisement in risky driving scenarios. Safety Science, 49, 1388-1393.

Meijering, B., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Verbrugge, R. (2012). What eye movements can tell about theory of mind in a strategic game. PLoS ONE 7(9): e45961. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045961.

Meital, N., Korinth, S. P., & Karni, A. (2013). Plasticity in the adult oculomotor system: Offline consolidation phase gains in saccade sequence learning. Brain Research, 1528, 42-48.

Melcher, D. (2008). Dynamic, object-based remapping of visual features in trans-saccadic perception. Journal of Vision, 8(14):2, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/2/, doi:10.1167/8.14.2.

Melcher, D., & Fracasso, A. (2012). Remapping of the line motion illusion across eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 503-514.

Melcher, D., & Piazza, M. (2011). The role of attentional priority and saliency in determining capacity limits in enumeration and visual working memory. PLoS ONE 6(12): e29296. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029296.

Menneer, T., Stroud, M. J., Cave, K. R., Li, X., Godwin, H. J., Liversedge, S. P., & Donnelly, N. (2012). Search for two categories of target produces fewer fixations to target-color items. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 404-418.

Mercer Moss, F. J., Baddeley, R., & Canagarajah, N. (2012). Eye movements to natural images as a function of sex and personality. PLoS ONE 7(11): e47870. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047870.

Mergenthaler, K., & Engbert, R. (2007). Modeling the control of fixational eye movements with neurophysiological delays. Physical Review Letters, 98, 138104.

Mergenthaler, K., & Engbert, R. (2010). Microsaccades are different from saccades in scene perception. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 753-757.

Merriam, E. P., Gardner, J. L., Movshon, J. A., & Heeger, D. J. (2013). Modulation of visual responses by gaze direction in human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 9879-9889.

Meyer, A. S., & van der Meulen, F. F. (2000). Phonological priming effects on speech onset latencies and viewing times in object naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 314-319.

Meyer, A. S., Belke, E., Häcker, C., & Mortensen, L. (2007). Use of word length information in utterance planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 210-231.

Meyer, A. S., Belke, E., Telling, A. L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2007). Early activation of object names in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 710-716.

Meyer, A. S., Ouellet, M., & Häcker, C. (2008). Parallel processing of objects in a naming task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 982-987.

Meyer, A. S., Roelofs, A., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2003). Word length effects in object naming: The role of a response criterion. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 131-147.

Meyer, A. S., Sleiderink, A. M., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1998). Viewing and naming objects: eye movements during noun phrase production. Cognition, 66, B25-B33.

Meyer, A. S., Wheeldon, L., van der Meulen, F., & Konopka, A. (2012). Effects of speech rate and practice on the allocation of visual attention in multiple object naming. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:39. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00039.

Meyer, A. S., van der Meulen, F. F., & Brooks, A. (2004). Eye movements during speech planning: Talking about present and remembered objects. Visual Cognition, 11, 553-576.

Meyer, K., Rasch, T., & Schnotz, W. (2010). Effects of animation's speed of presentation on perceptual processing and learning. Learning and Instruction, 20, 136-145.

Meyerhoff, H. S., Moeller, K., Debus, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2012). Multi-digit number processing beyond the two-digit number range: A combination of sequential and parallel processes. Acta Psychologica, 140, 81-90.

Michels, L., & Lappe, M. (2004). Contrast dependency of saccadic compression and suppression. Vision Research, 44, 2327-2336.

Miellet, S., Caldara, R., Schyns, P. G. (2011). Local Jekyll and global Hyde: The dual identity of face identification. Psychological Science, 22, 1518-1526.

Miellet, S., He, L., Zhou, X., Lao, J. & Caldara, R. (2012). When East meets West: gaze-contingent Blindspots abolish cultural diversity in eye movements for faces. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(2):5,1-12.

Miellet, S., O'Donnell, P. J., Sereno, S. C. (2009). Parafoveal magnification: Visual acuity does not modulate the perceptual span in reading. Psychological Science, 20, 721-728.

Miellet, S., Vizioli, L., He, L., Zhou, X., & Caldara, R (2013). Mapping face recognition information use across cultures. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:34. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00034.

Miellet, S., Zhou, X., He, L., Rodger, H., & Caldara, R. (2010). Investigating cultural diversity for extrafoveal information use in visual scenes. Journal of Vision, 10(6):21, 1-18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/21, doi:10.1167/10.6.21.

Mikulic, A., & Dorris, M. C. (2008). Temporal and spatial allocation of motor preparation during a mixed-strategy game. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 2101-2108.

Miller, J. E., Carlson, L. A., & McAuley, J. D. (2013). When what you hear influences when you see: listening to an auditory rhythm influences the temporal allocation of visual attention. Psychological Science, 24, 11-18.

Miller, L. M. S., & Cassady, D. L. (2012). Making healthy food choices using nutrition facts panels. The roles of knowledge, motivation, dietary modifications goals, and age. Appetite, 59, 129-139.

Miller, W. L., Maffei, V., Bosco, G., Iosa, M., Zago, M., Macaluso, E., & Lacquaniti, F. (2008). Vestibular nuclei and cerebellum put visual gravitational motion in context. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99, 1969-1982.

Mills, D. A., Frohman, T. C., Davis, S. L., Salter, A. R., McClure, S., Beatty, I., Shah, A., Galetta, S., Eggenberger, E., Zee, D. S., & Frohman, E. M. (2008). Break in binocular fusion during head turning in MS patients with INO. Neurology, 71, 458-460.

Mills, M., Hollingworth, A., Van der Stigchel, S., Hoffman, L., & Dodd, M. D. (2011). Examining the influence of task set on eye movements and fixations. Journal of Vision, 11(8):17, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/8/17,doi:10.1167/11.8.17.

Milosavljevic, M., Koch, C., & Rangel, A. (2011). Consumers can make decisions in as little as a third of a second. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 520-530.

Milosavljevic, M., Madsen, E., Koch, C., & Rangel, A. (2011). Fast saccades toward numbers: Simple number comparisons can be made in as little as 230 ms. Journal of Vision, 11(4):4, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/4/4, doi:10.1167/11.4.4.

Milosavljevic, M., Malmaud, J., Huth, A., Koch, C., & Rangel, A. (2010). The drift diffusion model can account for the accuracy and reaction times of value-based choice under high and low time pressure. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 437-449.

Milosavljevic, M., Navalpakkam, V., Koch, C., & Rangel, A. (2012). Relative visual saliency differences induce sizable bias in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 67-74.

Milstein, D. M., & Dorris, M. C. (2007). The influence of expected value on saccadic preparation. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 4810-4818.

Milstein, D. M., & Dorris, M. C. (2011). The relationship between saccadic choice and reaction times with manipulations of target value. Frontiers in Neurosciences, 5:122. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00122.

Mineault, P. J., Khawaja, F. A., Butts, D. A., & Pack, C. C. (2012). Hierarchical processing of complex motion along the primate dorsal visual pathway Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, E972-E980.

Mineault, P.J., Zanos, T.P., & Pack, C.C. (2013). Local field potentials reflect multiple spatial scales in V4. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 7:21. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00021.

Mirman, D., & Graziano, K. M. (2012). Damage to temporo-parietal cortex decreases incidental activation of thematic relations during spoken word comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1990-1997.

Mirman, D., & Graziano, K. M. (2012). Individual differences in the strength of taxonomic versus thematic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 601-609.

Mirman, D., Britt, A. E., & Chen, Q. (2013). Effects of phonological and semantic deficits on facilitative and inhibitory consequences of item repetition in spoken word comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1848-1856.

Mirman, D., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E., & Magnuson, J. S. (2011). Theories of spoken word recognition deficits in Aphasia: Evidence from eye-tracking and computational modeling. Brain and Language, 117, 53-68.

Mital, P., Smith, T., Hill, R., & Henderson, J. (2011). Clustering of gaze during dynamic scene viewing is predicted by motion. Cognitive Computation, 3, 5-24.

Mitchell, D. C., Shen, X., Green, M. J., & Hodgson, T. L. (2008). Accounting for regressive eye-movements in models of sentence processing: A reappraisal of the Selective Reanalysis hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 266-293.

Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 95-103.

Mitra, A. R., Abegg, M., Viswanathan, J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2010). Line bisection in simulated homonymous hemianopia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1742-1749.

Mitterer, H. (2011). The mental lexicon is fully specified: Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 496-513.

Mitterer, H., & McQueen, J. M. (2009). Processing reduced word-forms in speech perception using probabilistic knowledge about speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 244-263.

Mitterer, H., & Reinisch, E. (2013). No delays in application of perceptual learning in speech recognition: Evidence from eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 527-545.

Mitterer, H., & Russell, K. (2013). How phonological reductions sometimes help the listener. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 977-984.

Mitterer, H., Kim, S., & Cho, T. (2013). Compensation for complete assimilation in speech perception: The case of Korean labial-to-velar assimilation. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 59-83.

Moates, A. F., Ivleva, E. I., O'Neill, H. B., Krishna, N., Cullum, C. M., Thaker, G. K., & Tamminga, C. A. (2012). Predictive pursuit association with deficits in working memory in psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 752-757.

Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2009). Sequential or parallel decomposed processing of two-digit numbers? Evidence from eye-tracking. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 323-334.

Moeller, K., Klein, E., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). (No) small adults: Children's processing of carry addition problems. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 702-720.

Moeller, K., Klein, E., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Three processes underlying the carry effect in addition - Evidence from eye tracking. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 623-645.

Moeller, K., Neuburger, S., Kaufmann, L., Landerl, K., Nuerk, H.-C. (2009). Basic number processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognitive Development, 24, 371-386.

Mohammad, S., Gottlob, I., Kumar, A., Thomas, M., Degg, C., Sheth, V., & Proudlock, F. A. (2011). The functional significance of foveal abnormalities in albinism measured using spectral-domain optical coherence tomography. Ophthalmology, 118, 1645-1652.

Moher, J., Abrams, J., Egeth, H. E., Yantis, S., & Stuphorn, V. (2011). Trial-by-trial adjustments of top-down set modulate oculomotor capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 897-903.

Mojzisch, A., Schilbach, L., Helmert, J. R., Pannasch, S., Velichkovsky, B. M., & Vogeley, K. (2006). The effects of self-involvement on attention, arousal, and facial expression during social interaction with virtual others: A psychophysiological study. Social Neuroscience, 1, 184-195.

Moll, K. & Jones, M. (2013). Naming fluency in dyslexic and nondyslexic readers: Differential effects of visual crowding in foveal, parafoveal, and peripheral vision. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2085-2091.

Montagnini, A., & Castet, E. (2007). Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. Journal of Vision, 7(14):8, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/7/14/8/, doi:10.1167/7.14.8.

Montagnini, A., & Chelazzi, L. (2005). The urgency to look: Prompt saccades to the benefit of perception. Vision Research, 45, 3391-3401.

Montagnini, A., & Chelazzi, L. (2009). Dynamic interaction between "Go" and "Stop" signals in the saccadic eye movement system: New evidence against the functional independence of the underlying neural mechanisms. Vision Research, 49, 1316-1328.

Montfoort, I., Frens, M. A., Hooge, I. Th. C., Lagers-van Haselen, G. C., & van der Geest, J. N. (2007). Visual search deficits in Williams-Beuren syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45, 931-938.

Montfoort, I., Kelders, W. P., van der Geest, J. N., Schipper, I. B., Feenstra, L., de Zeeuw, C. I., & Frens, M. A. (2006). Interaction between ocular stabilization reflexes in patients with whiplash injury. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 47, 2881-2884.

Montfoort, I., Van Der Geest, J. N., Slijper, H. P., De Zeeuw, C. I., & Frens, M. A. (2008). Adaptation of the cervico- and vestibulo-ocular reflex in whiplash injury patients. Journal of Neurotrauma, 25, 687-693.

Monti, J. M., Hillman, C. H., & Cohen, N. J. (2012). Aerobic fitness enhances relational memory in preadolescent children: The FITKids randomized control trial. Hippocampus, 22, 1876-1882.

Moores, E., Laiti, L., & Chelazzi, L. (2003). Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 182-189.

Morand, S. M., Grosbras, M.-H., Caldara, R., & Harvey, M. (2010). Looking away from faces: Influence of high-level visual processes on saccade programming. Journal of Vision, 10(3):16, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/10/3/16/, doi:10.1167/10.3.16.

Morein-Zamir, S., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Fixation offset and stop signal intensity effects on saccadic countermanding: A crossmodal investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 453-462.

Morel, P., Deneve, S., & Baraduc, P. (2011). Optimal and suboptimal use of postsaccadic vision in sequences of saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 10039-10049.

Moresi, S., Adam, J. J., Rijcken, J., & Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2008). Cue validity effects in response preparation: A pupillometric study. Brain Research, 1196, 94-102.

Moresi, S., Adam, J. J., Rijcken, J., Kuipers, H., Severens, M., & Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2011). Response preparation with adjacent versus overlapped hands: A pupillometric study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79, 280-286.

Moresi, S., Adam, J. J., Rijcken, J., Van Gerven, P. W. M., Kuipers, H., & Jolles, J. (2008). Pupil dilation in response preparation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 67, 124-130.

Morgan, C. J. A., Huddy, V., Lipton, M., Curran, H. V., & Joyce, E. M. (2009). Is persistent ketamine use a valid model of the cognitive and oculomotor deficits in schizophrenia? Biological Psychiatry, 65, 1099-1102.

Morgan, J. L., & Meyer, A. S. (2005). Processing of extrafoveal objects during multiple-object naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 428-442.

Morgan, J. L., van Elswijk, G., & Meyer, A. S. (2008). Extrafoveal processing of objects in a naming task: Evidence from word probe experiments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 561-565.

Morris, A. P., Liu, C. C., Cropper, S. J., Forte, J. D., Krekelberg, B., & Mattingley, J. B. (2010). Summation of visual motion across eye movements reflects a nonspatial decision mechanism. Jource of Neuroscience, 30, 9821-9830.

Morrow, D., D'andrea, L., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Shake, M., Bertel, S., Chin, J., Kopren, K., Gao, X., Conner-Garcia, T., Graumlich, J., & Murray, M. (2012). Comprehension of multimedia health information among older adults with chronic illness. Visual Communication, 11, 347-362.

Mort, D. J., Perry, R. J., Mannan, S. K., Hodgson, T. L., Anderson, E., Quest, R., McRobbie, D., McBride, A., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2003). Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. NeuroImage, 18, 231-246.

Mortensen, L., Meyer, A. S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Speech planning during multiple-object naming: Effects of ageing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1217-1238.

Mortier, K., Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2003). Attentional capture within and between objects. Acta Psychologica, 113, 133-145.

Mortier, K., van Zoest, W., Meeter, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Word cues affect detection but not localization responses. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 65-75.

Morvan, C., & Maloney, L. T. (2012). Human visual search does not maximize the post-saccadic probability of identifying targets. PLoS Computational Biology, 8(2): e1002342. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002342.

Morvan, C., & Wexler, M. (2009). The nonlinear structure of motion perception during smooth eye movements. Journal of Vision, 9(7):1, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/9/7/1/, doi:10.1167/9.7.1.

Moshel, S., Liang, J.-R., Caspi, A., Engbert, R., Kliegl, R., Havlin, S., & Zivotofsky, A.Z. (2005). Phase-synchronization decay of fixational eye movements. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 484-488.

Moshel, S., Zivotofsky, A. Z., Liang, J.-R., Engbert, R., Kurths, J., Kliegl, R., & Havlin, S. (2008). Persistence and phase synchronisation properties of fixational eye movements. The European Physical Journal, 161, 1951-6355.

Mosimann, U. P., Felblinger, J., Ballinari, P., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2004). Visual exploration behaviour during clock reading in Alzheimer's disease. Brain, 127, 431-438.

Motoyoshi, I. (2011). Attentional modulation of temporal contrast sensitivity in human vision. PLoS ONE 6(4): e19303. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019303.

Motter, B. C., & Simoni, D. A. (2007). The roles of cortical image separation and size in active visual search performance. Journal of Vision, 7(2):6, 1-15, http://journalofvision.org/7/2/6/, doi:10.1167/7.2.6.

Motter, B. C., & Simoni, D. A. (2008). Changes in the functional visual field during search with and without eye movements. Vision Research, 48, 2382-2393.

Mou, W., Liu, L., & McNamara, T. P. (2009). Layout geometry in encoding and retrieval of spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 83-93.

Moukheiber, A., Rautureau, G., Perez-Diaz, F., Jouvent, R., & Pelissolo, A. (2012). Gaze behaviour in social blushers. Psychiatry Research, 200, 614-619.

Moukheiber, A., Rautureau, G., Perez-Diaz, F., Soussignan, R., Dubal, S., Jouvent, R., & Pelissolo, A. (2010). Gaze avoidance in social phobia: Objective measure and correlates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 147-151.

Moutoussis, K., Keliris, G., Kourtzi, Z., & Logothetis, N. (2005). A binocular rivalry study of motion perception in the human brain. Vision Research, 45, 2231-2243.

Moutsiana, C., Field, D. T., & Harris, J. P. (2011). The neural basis of centre-surround interactions in visual motion processing. PLoS ONE 6(7): e22902. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022902.

Moxey, L. M., & Filik, R. (2010). The effects of character desire on focus patterns and pronominal reference following quantified statements. Discourse Processes, 47, 588-616.

Mozuraitis, M., Chambers, C. G., & Daneman, M. (2013). Younger and older adults' use of verb aspect and world knowledge in the online interpretation of discourse. Discourse Processes, 50, 1-22.

Mrotek, L. A. (2013). Following and intercepting scribbles: interactions between eye and hand control. Experimental Brain Research, 227, 161-174.

Mrotek, L. A., & Soechting, J. F. (2007). Predicting curvilinear target motion through an occlusion. Experimental Brain Research, 178, 99-114.

Mrotek, L. A., & Soechting, J. F. (2007). Target interception: Hand-eye coordination and strategies. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 7297-7309.

Mrotek, L. A., Flanders, M., & Soechting, J. F. (2004). Interception of targets using brief directional cues. Experimental Brain Research, 156, 94-103.

Mrotek, L. A., Flanders, M., & Soechting, J. F. (2006). Oculomotor responses to gradual changes in target direction. Experimental Brain Research, 172, 175-192.

Mruczek, R. E. B., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2007). Activity of inferior temporal cortical neurons predicts recognition choice behavior and recognition time during visual search. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 2825-2836.

Mruczek, R. E. B., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2007). Context familiarity enhances target processing by inferior temporal cortex neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8533-8545.

Mruczek, R. E. B., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2012). Stimulus selectivity and response latency in putative inhibitory and excitatory neurons of the primate inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 2725-2736.

Mucke, S., Manahilov, V., Strang, N. C., Seidel, D., Gray, L. S., & Shahani, U. (2010). Investigating the mechanisms that may underlie the reduction in contrast sensitivity during dynamic accommodation. Journal of Vision, 10(5):5, 1-14, http://journalofvision.org/content/10/5/5, doi:10.1167/10.5.5.

Mucke, S., Strang, N. C., Aydin, S., Mallen, E. A. H., Seidel, D., & Manahilov, V. (2013). Spatial frequency selectivity of visual suppression during convergence eye movements. Vision Research,89, 96-101.

Muggleton, N .G., Kalla, R., Juan, C.H., & Walsh, V. (2011). Dissociating the contributions of human frontal eye fields and posterior parietal cortex to visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 2891-2896.

Mulckhuyse, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Unconscious cueing effects in saccadic eye movements - Facilitation and inhibition in temporal and nasal hemifield. Vision Research, 50, 606-613.

Mulckhuyse, M., Crombez, G., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2013). Conditioned fear modulates visual selection. Emotion, 13, 529-536.

Mulckhuyse, M., Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). Early and late modulation of saccade deviations by target distractor similarity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 1451-1458.

Mulckhuyse, M., van Zoest, W., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Capture of the eyes by relevant and irrelevant onsets. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 225-235.

Munafò, M. R., Roberts, N., Bauld, L., Leonards, U. (2011). Plain packaging increases visual attention to health warnings on cigarette packs in non-smokers and weekly smokers but not daily smokers. Addiction, 106, 1505-1510.

Munuera, J., Morel, P., Duhamel, J.-R., & Deneve, S. (2009). Optimal sensorimotor control in eye movement sequences. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3026-3035.

Murphy, P. R., Robertson, I. H., Allen, D., Hester, R., & O'Connell, R. G. (2012). An electrophysiological signal that precisely tracks the emergence of error awareness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6:65.

Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2013). Visual and linguistic cues to graspable objects. Experimental Brain Research, 229, 545-559.

Myachykov, A., Garrod, S., & Scheepers, C. (2012). Determinants of structural choice in visually situated sentence production. Acta Psychologica, 141, 304-315.

Myachykov, A., Scheepers, C., Garrod, S., Thompson, D., & Fedorova, O. (2013). Syntactic flexibility and competition in sentence production: The case of English and Russian. The Quarterly Journal Experimental Psychology, 66, 1601-1619.

Myachykov, A., Thompson, D., Garrod, S., & Scheepers, C. (2012). Referential and visual cues to structural choice in visually situated sentence production. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:396. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00396.

Myung, J.-Y., Blumstein, S. E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: Manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition, 98, 223-243.

Myung, J.-Y., Blumstein, S. E., Yee, E., Sedivy, J. C., Thompson-Schill, S. L., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2010). Impaired access to manipulation features in Apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking and semantic judgment tasks. Brain and Language, 112, 101-112.

Mäntylä, T., & Holm, L. (2005). Remembering parts and wholes: Configural processing in face recollection. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 753-769.

Mäntylä, T., & Holm, L. (2006). Gaze control and recollective experience in face recognition. Visual Cognition, 14, 365-386.

Møller, F., Laursen, M. L., & Sjølie, A. K. (2005). Binocular fixation topography in patients with diabetic macular oedema: Possible implications for photocoagulation therapy (3rd revision). Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 243, 903-910.

Møller, F., Laursen, M. L., & Sjølie, A. K. (2006). Fixation topography in normal test persons. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 244, 577-582.

Møller, F., Laursen, M. L., & Sjølie, A. K. (2006). The contribution of microsaccades and drifts in the maintenance of binocular steady fixation. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 244, 465-471.

Møller, F., Laursen, M., Tygesen, J., & Sjølie, A. (2002). Binocular quantification and characterization of microsaccades. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 240, 765-770.

Müller, M. G., Kappas, A., & Olk, B. (2012). Perceiving press photography: a new integrative model, combining iconology with psychophysiological and eye-tracking methods. Visual Communication, 11, 307-328.

Müller, R., Helmert, J. R., Pannasch, S. & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2013). Gaze transfer in remote cooperation: Is it always helpful to see what your partner is attending to? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1302-1316.

Müri, R. M., Cazzoli, D., Nyffeler, T., & Pflugshaupt, T. (2009). Visual exploration pattern in hemineglect. Psychological Research, 73, 147-157.

Müsseler, J., & Tiggelbeck, J. (2013). The perceived onset position of a moving target: Effects of trial contexts are evoked by different attentional allocations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 349-357.

Back To Top

N

Naber, M., & Nakayama, K. (2013). Pupil responses to high-level image content. Journal of Vision, 13(6):7, 1-8, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/6/7, doi:10.1167/13.6.7.

Naber, M., Alvarez, G.A., & Nakayama, K. (2013). Tracking the allocation of attention using human pupillary oscillations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:919. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00919.

Naber, M., Frassle, S., Rutishauser, U., & Einhauser, W. (2013). Pupil size signals novelty and predicts later retrieval success for declarative memories of natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 13(2):11, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/2/X11, doi:10.1167/13.2.11.

Naber, M., Frässle, S., & Einhäuser, W. (2011). Perceptual rivalry: Reflexes reveal the gradual nature of visual awareness. PLoS ONE 6(6): e20910. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020910.

Naber, M., Hilger, M., & Einhäuser, W. (2012). Animal detection and identification in natural scenes: Image statistics and emotional valence. Journal of Vision, 12(1):25, 1-24, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/1/25, doi:10.1167/12.1.25.

Nakabayashi, K., Lloyd-Jones, T. J., Butcher, N., & Liu, C.-H. (2012). Independent influences of verbalization and race on the configural and featural processing of faces: A behavioral and eye movement study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 61-77.

Nakamura, C., Arai, M., & Mazuka, R. (2012). Immediate use of prosody and context in predicting a syntactic structure. Cognition, 125, 317-323.

Nakano, T., Higashida, N., & Kitazawa, S. (2013). Facilitation of face recognition through the retino-tectal pathway. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2043-2049.

Nakatani, C., & Pollatsek, A. (2004). An eye movement analysis of "mental rotation" of simple scenes. Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 1227-1245.

Nakatani, C., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). A pragmatic approach to multi-modality in fixation duration studies of cognitive processes. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(2):2, 1-12.

Nakatani, C., Chehelcheraghi, M., Jarrahi, B., Nakatani, H., & van Leeuwen, C. (2013). Cross-frequency phase synchrony around the saccade period as a correlate of perceiver's internal state. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:18. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00018.

Nakatani, H., & Van Leeuwen, C. (2013). Antecedent occipital alpha band activity predicts the impact of oculomotor events in perceptual switching. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00019.

Nakatani, H., Orlandi, N., & van Leeuwen, C. (2011). Precisely timed oculomotor and parietal EEG activity in perceptual switching. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 5, 399-409.

Nakayama, M., Sears, C. R., & Lupker, S. J. (2010). Testing for lexical competition during reading: Fast priming with orthographic neighbors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 36, 477-492.

Nasr, S., & Tootell, R. B. H. (2012). A cardinal orientation bias in scene-selective visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 14921-14926.

Nater, F., Vangeneugden, J., Grabner, H., Van Gool, L., & Vogels, R. (2012). Discrimination of Locomotion Direction at Different Speeds: A Comparison between Macaque Monkeys and Algorithms. Studies in Computational Intelligence, 384, 181-190.

Nau, A., Hertle, R. W., & Yang, D. (2012). Effect of tongue stimulation on nystagmus eye movements in blind patients. Brain Structure and Function, 217, 761-765.

Naue, N., Strüber, D., Fründ, I., Schadow, J., Lenz, D., Rach, S., Körner, U., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Gamma in motion: Pattern reversal elicits stronger gamma-band responses than motion. NeuroImage, 55, 808-817.

Navalpakkam, V., Koch, C., Rangel, A., & Perona, P. (2010). Optimal reward harvesting in complex perceptual environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 5232-5237.

Nazir, T. A., Ben-Boutayab, N., Decoppet, N., Deutsch, A., & Frost, R. (2004). Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words. Brain and Language, 88, 294-311.

Neath, K. N., & Itier, R. J. (2014). Facial expression discrimination varies with presentation time but not with fixation on features: A backward masking study using eye-tracking. Cognition & Emotion, 28, 115-131.

Nefs, H. T., & Harris, J. M. (2007). Vergence effects on the perception of motion-in-depth. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 313-322.

Nefs, H. T., & Harris, J. M. (2008). Induced motion in depth and the effects of vergence eye movements. Journal of Vision, 8(3):8, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/8/3/8/, doi:10.1167/8.3.8.

Neggers, S. F. W., & Bekkering, H. (1999). Integration of visual and somatosensory target information in goal-directed eye and arm movements. Experimental Brain Research, 125, 97-107.

Neggers, S. F. W., & Bekkering, H. (2000). Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. The Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 639-651.

Neggers, S. F. W., & Bekkering, H. (2001). Gaze anchoring to a pointing target Is present during the entire pointing movement and Is driven by a non-visual signal. The Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 961-970.

Neggers, S. F. W., & Bekkering, H. (2002). Coordinated control of eye and hand movements in dynamic reaching. Human Movement Science, 21, 37-64.

Neggers, S. F. W., Huijbers, W., Vrijlandt, C. M., Vlaskamp, B. N. S., Schutter, D. J. L. G., & Kenemans, J. L. (2007). TMS pulses on the frontal eye fields break coupling between visuo-spatial attention and eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 2765-2778.

Neggers, S. F. W., Schölvinck, M. L., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Postma, A. (2005). Quantifying the interactions between allo- and egocentric representations of space. Acta Psychologica, 118, 25-45.

Neider, M. B., & Kramer, A. F. (2011). Older adults capitalize on contextual information to guide search. Experimental Aging Research, 37, 539-571.

Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Scene context guides eye movements during visual search. Vision Research, 46, 614-621.

Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Searching for camouflaged targets: Effects of target-background similarity on visual search. Vision Research, 46, 2217-2235.

Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). Exploring set size effects in scenes: Identifying the objects of search. Visual Cognition, 16, 1-10.

Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2011). Cutting through the clutter: Searching for targets in evolving complex scenes. Journal of Vision, 11(14):7, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/7, doi:10.1167/11.14.7.

Neider, M. B., Boot, W. R., & Kramer, A. F. (2010). Visual search for real world targets under conditions of high target-background similarity: Exploring training and transfer in younger and older adults. Acta Psychologica, 134, 29-39.

Neider, M. B., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A, Brennan, S. E, & Zelinsky, G. J. (2010). Coordinating spatial referencing using shared gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 718-724.

Neumann, D., Spezio, M. L., Piven, J., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Looking you in the mouth: abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 194-202.

Neveu, P., Philippe, M., Priot, A. E., Fuchs, P., Roumes, C. (2012). Vergence tracking: a tool to assess oculomotor performance in stereoscopic displays. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(2):1, 1-8.

Nguyen, N. X., Stockum, A., Hahn, G. A., & Trauzettel-Klosinski, S. (2011). Training to improve reading speed in patients with juvenile macular dystrophy: a randomized study comparing two training methods. Acta Ophthalmologica, 89, e82-e88.

Ni, J., Jiang, H., Jin, Y., Chen, N., Wang, J., Wang, Z., Luo, Y., Ma, Y., & Hu, X. (2011). Dissociable modulation of overt visual attention in valence and arousal revealed by topology of scan path. PLoS ONE 6(4): e18262. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018262.

Niebergall, R., Khayat, P. S., Treue, S., & Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2011). Expansion of MT neurons excitatory receptive fields during covert attentive tracking. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 15499-15510.

Niebergall, R., Khayat, P. S., Treue, S., & Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2011). Multifocal attention filters targets from distracters within and beyond primate MT neurons' receptive field Boundaries. Neuron, 72, 1067-1079.

Nieman, D., Sheth, B. R., & Shimojo, S. (2010). Perceiving a discontinuity in motion. Journal of Vision, 10(6):9, 1-23, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/9, doi:10.1167/10.6.9.

Niemann, T., Lappe, M., Büscher, A., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (1999). Ocular responses to radial optic flow and single accelerated targets in humans. Vision Research, 39, 1359-1371.

Nijboer, T. C. W., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2009). Is attention essential for inducing synesthetic colors? Evidence from oculomotor distractors. Journal of Vision, 9(6):21, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/9/6/21/, doi:10.1167/9.6.21.

Nijboer, T. C. W., Satris, G., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2011). The influence of synesthesia on eye movements: No synesthetic pop-out in an oculomotor target selection task. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1193-1200.

Nijboer, T. C. W., Vree, A., Dijkerman, H. C., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2010). Prism adaptation influences perception but not attention: evidence from antisaccades. NeuroReport, 21, 386-389.

Nikolaev, A. R., Nakatani, C., Plomp, G., Jurica, P., & van Leeuwen, C. (2011). Eye fixation-related potentials in free viewing identify encoding failures in change detection. NeuroImage, 56, 1598-1607.

Nikolaev, A.R., Jurica, P., Nakatani, C., Plomp, G., & van Leeuwen, C. (2013). Visual encoding and fixation target selection in free viewing: Presaccadic brain potentials. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:26. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00026.

Nishida, S., Shibata, T., & Ikeda, K. (2009). Prediction of human eye movements in facial discrimination tasks. Artificial Life and Robotics, 14, 348-351.

Nishimoto, S., & Gallant, J. L. (2011). A three-dimensional spatiotemporal receptive field model explains responses of area MT neurons to naturalistic movies. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 14551-14564.

Niu, Y., Todd, R. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Affective salience can reverse the effects of stimulus-driven salience on eye movements in complex scenes. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00336.

Niu, Y.-Q., Xiao, Q., Liu, R.-F., Wu, L.-Q., & Wang, S.-R. (2006). Response characteristics of pretectal neurons to illusory contours and motion. Journal of Physiology, 577, 805-813.

Niwa, M., & Ditterich, J. (2008). Perceptual decisions between multiple directions of visual motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 4435-4445.

Noguchi, Y., Shimojo, S., Kakigi, R., & Hoshiyama, M. (2007). Spatial contexts can inhibit a mislocalization of visual stimuli during smooth pursuit. Journal of Vision, 7(13):13, 1-15, http://journalofvision.org/7/13/13/, doi:10.1167/7.13.13.

Noguchi, Y., Shimojo, S., Kakigi, R., & Hoshiyama, M. (2011). An integration of color and motion information in visual scene analyses. Psychological Science, 22, 153-158.

Noh, E.-J., Choo, H., & Koh, S. (2013). Processing metalinguistic negation: Evidence from eye-tracking experiments. Journal of Pragmatics, 57, 1-18.

Noritake, A., Kazai, K., Terao, M., & Yagi, A. (2005). A continuously lit stimulus is perceived to be shorter than a flickering stimulus during a saccade. Spatial Vision, 18, 297-316.

Noritake, A., Uttl, B., Terao, M., Nagai, M., Watanabe., J., & Yagi, A. (2009). Saccadic compression of rectangle and kanizsa figures: Now you see it, now you don't. PLoS ONE 4(7): e6383. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006383.

Nottbusch, G. (2010). Grammatical planning, execution, and control in written sentence production. Reading and Writing, 23, 777-801.

Novick, L. R., Stull, A. T., & Catley, K. M. (2012). Reading phylogenetic trees: The effects of tree orientation and text processing on comprehension. BioScience, 62, 757-764.

Nuding, U., Kalla, R., Muggleton, N. G., Büttner, U., Walsh, V., & Glasauer, S. (2009). TMS Evidence for smooth pursuit gain control by the frontal eye fields. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1144-1150.

Nummenmaa, L., & Hietanen, J. K. (2006). Gaze distractors influence saccadic curvature: Evidence for the role of the oculomotor system in gaze-cued orienting. Vision Research, 46, 3674-3680.

Nummenmaa, L., Hietanen, J. K., Calvo, M. G., & Hyönä, J. (2011). Food catches the eye but not for everyone: A BMI-contingent attentional bias in rapid detection of nutriments. PLoS ONE 6(5): e19215. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019215.

Nummenmaa, L., Hietanen, J. K., Santtila, P., & Hyönä, J. (2012). Gender and visibility of sexual cues influence eye movements while viewing faces and bodies. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 1439-1451.

Nummenmaa, L., Hirvonen, J., Parkkola, R., & Hietanen, J. K. (2008). Is emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for affective and cognitive empathy. NeuroImage, 43, 571-580.

Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G (2010). Semantic categorization precedes affective evaluation of visual scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 222-246.

Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6, 257-268.

Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2009). Emotional scene content drives the saccade generation system reflexively. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 305-323.

Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Hietanen, J. (2009). I'll walk this way: Eyes reveal the direction of locomotion and make passers-by to look and go the other way. Psychological Science, 20, 1454-1458.

Nuthmann, A. (2013). Not fixating at the line of text comes at a cost. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1604-1609.

Nuthmann, A. (2013). On the visual span during object search in real-world scenes. Visual Cognition, 21, 803-837.

Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2009). Mindless reading revisited: An analysis based on the SWIFT model of eye-movement control. Vision Research, 49, 322-336.

Nuthmann, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2010). Object-based attentional selection in scene viewing. Journal of Vision, 10(8):20, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/20, doi:10.1167/10.8.20.

Nuthmann, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2012). Using CRISP to model global characteristics of fixation durations in scene viewing and reading with a common mechanism. Visual Cognition, 20, 457-494.

Nuthmann, A., & Kliegl, R. (2009). An examination of binocular reading fixations based on sentence corpus data. Journal of Vision, 9(5):31, 1-28, http://journalofvision.org/9/5/31/, doi:10.1167/9.5.31.

Nuthmann, A., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Mislocated fixations during reading and the inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision Research, 45, 2201-2217.

Nuthmann, A., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2007). The IOVP effect in mindless reading: Experiment and modeling. Vision Research, 47, 990-1002.

Nuthmann, A., Smith, T. J., Engbert, R., & Henderson, J. M. (2010). CRISP: A computational model of fixation durations in scene viewing. Psychological Review, 117, 382-405.

Nyamsuren, E., & Taatgen, N. A. (2013). Set as an instance of a real-world visual-cognitive task. Cognitive Science, 37, 146-175.

Nyamsuren, E., & Taatgen, N.A. (2013). The effect of visual representation style in problem-solving: A perspective from cognitive processes. PLoS ONE 8(11): e80550. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080550.

Nyffeler, T., Cazzoli, D., Wurtz, P., Luthi, M., von Wartburg, R., Chaves, S., Deruaz, A., Hess, C. W., & Muri, R. M. (2008). Neglect-like visual exploration behaviour after theta burst transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right posterior parietal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1809-1813.

Nyffeler, T., Pflugshaupt, T., Hofer, H., Baas, U., Gutbrod, K., von Wartburg, R., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2005). Oculomotor behaviour in simultanagnosia: A longitudinal case study. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1591-1597.

Näsänen, R., & Ojanpää, H. (2003). Effect of image contrast and sharpness on visual search for computer icons. Displays, 24, 137-144.

Näsänen, R., & Ojanpää, H. (2004). How many faces can be processed during a single eye fixation? Perception, 33, 67-77.

Näsänen, R., Karlsson, J., & Ojanpää, H. (2001). Display quality and the speed of visual letter search. Displays, 22, 107-113.

Näsänen, R., Ojanpää, H., & Kojo, I. (2001). Effect of stimulus contrast on performance and eye movements in visual search. Vision Research, 41, 1817-1824.

Back To Top

O

O'Connor, E., Margrain, T. H., & Freeman, T. C. A. (2010). Age, eye movement and motion discrimination. Vision Research, 50, 2588-2599.

O'Driscoll, G. A., Benkelfat, C., Florencio, P., Wolff, A. L. V., Joober, R., Lal, S., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Neural correlates of eye-tracking deficits in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients: a PET study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 1127-1134.

O'Driscoll, G. A., Dépatie, L., Holahan, A. V., Savion-Lemieux, T., Barr, R. G., Jolicoeur, C., & Douglas, V. I. (2005). Executive functions and methylphenidate response in subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1452-1460.

O'Driscoll, G. A., Wolff, A. L. V., Benkelfat, C., Florencio, P. S., Lal, S., & Evans, A. C. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades. NeuroReport, 11, 1335-1340.

O'Hare, L., Clarke, A. D., & Hibbard, P. B. (2013). Visual search and visual discomfort. Perception, 42, 1-15.

O'Reilly, J.X., Schüffelgen, U., Cuell, S.F., Behrens, T.E., Mars, R.B., & Rushworth, M.F. (2013). Dissociable effects of surprise and model update in parietal and anterior cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, E3660-E3669;

O'Shea, J., Muggleton, N. G., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2007). Human frontal eye fields and spatial priming of pop-out. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1140-1151.

Odoj, B., & Balslev, D. (2013). Visual sensitivity shifts with perceived eye position. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1180-1189.

Ogawa, H., & Watanabe, K. (2010). Time to learn: Evidence for two types of attentional guidance in contextual cueing. Perception, 39, 72-80.

Ogun, O., Viswanathan, J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2011). The effect of central (macula) sparing of contralateral line bisection bias: A study with virtual hemianopia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3377-3382.

Ohl, S., Brandt, S. A., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Secondary (micro-)saccades: The influence of primary saccade end point and target eccentricity on the process of postsaccadic fixation. Vision Research, 51, 2340-2347.

Ohl, S., Brandt, S. A., & Kliegl, R. (2013) The generation of secondary saccades without postsaccadic visual feedback. Journal of Vision, 13(5):11, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/5/11, doi:10.1167/13.5.11.

Ojanpää, H., & Näsänen, R. (2003). Effects of luminance and colour contrast on the search of information on display devices. Displays, 24, 167-178.

Ojanpää, H., & Näsänen, R. (2003). Utilisation of spatial frequency information in face search. Vision Research, 43, 2505-2515.

Ojanpää, H., Näsänen, R., & Kojo, I. (2002). Eye movements in the visual search of word lists. Vision Research, 42, 1499-1512.

Ojanpää, H., Näsänen, R., Päällysaho, J., Akila, R., Müller, K., Kaukiainen, A., & Sainio, M. (2006). Visual search and eye movements in patients with chronic solvent-induced toxic encephalopathy. NeuroToxicology, 27, 1013-1023.

Oleksiak, A., Klink, P. C., Postma, A., van der Ham, I. J. M., Lankheet, M. J., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2011). Spatial summation in macaque parietal area 7a follows a winner-take-all rule. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 1150-1158.

Oleksiak, A., Manko, M., Postma, A., van der Ham, I. J. M., van den Berg, A. V., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2010). Distance estimation is influenced by encoding conditions. PLoS ONE 5(3): e9918. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009918

Olivers, C. N. L., Meijer, F., & Theeuwes, J. (2006). Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: Visual working memory content affects visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 1243-1265.

Olk, B. (2013). Measuring the allocation of attention in the Stroop task: evidence from eye movement patterns. Psychological Research, 77, 106-115.

Olk, B., & Jin, Y. (2011). Effects of aging on switching the response direction of pro- and antisaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 139-150.

Olk, B., & Kingstone, A. (2009). A new look at aging and performance in the antisaccade task: The impact of response selection. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 406-427 .

Olk, B., Harvey, M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). First saccades reveal biases in recovered neglect. Neurocase, 8, 306-313.

Onat, S., Libertus, K., & König, P. (2007). Integrating audiovisual information for the control of overt attention. Journal of Vision, 7(10):11, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/7/10/11/, doi:10.1167/7.10.11.

Ooms, K., Andrienko, G., Andrienko, N., De Maeyer, P., & Fack, V. (2012). Analysing the spatial dimension of eye movement data using a visual analytic approach. Expert Systems with Applications, 39, 1324-1332.

Ooms, K., De Maeyer, P., & Fack, V. (2014). Study of the attentive behavior of novice and expert map users using eye tracking. Cartography and Geographic Information Science, 41, 37-54.

Ooms, K., De Maeyer, P., Fack, V., Van Assche, E., & Witlox, F. (2012). Interpreting maps through the eyes of expert and novice users. International Journal of Geographical Information Science, 26, 1773-1788.

Ooms, K., de Maeyer, P., Fack, V., van Assche, E., & Witlox, F. (2012). Investigating the effectiveness of an efficient label placement method using eye movement data. The Cartographic Journal, 49, 234-246.

Op de Beeck, H. P., Deutsch, J. A., Vanduffel, W., Kanwisher, N. G., & DiCarlo, J. J. (2008). A stable topography of selectivity for unfamiliar shape classes in monkey inferior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1676-1694.

Op de Beeck, H.P., Vermaercke, B., Woolley, D.G., & Wenderoth, N. (2013). Combinatorial brain decoding of people's whereabouts during visuospatial navigation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7:78. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00078.

Opris, I., Hampson, R. E., Gerhardt, G. A., Berger, T. W., & Deadwyler, S. A. (2012). Columnar processing in primate pFC: Evidence for executive control microcircuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 2334-2347.

Orban de Xivry, J.-J., Coppe, S., Lefèvre, P., & Missal, M. (2010). Biological motion drives perception and action. Journal of Vision, 10(2):6, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/10/2/6/, doi:10.1167/10.2.6.

Ossandon, J. P., Onat, S., Cazzoli, D., Nyffeler, T., Muri, R., & Konig, Peter (2012). Unmasking the contribution of low-level features to the guidance of attention. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3478-3487.

Ossandón, J. P., Helo, A. V., Montefusco-Siegmund, R., & Maldonado, P. E. (2010). Superposition model predicts EEG occipital activity during free viewing of natural scenes. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4787-4795.

Ossandón, J. P., Onat, S., & Cazzoli, D., Nyffeler, T., Müri, R., & König, P. (2012). Unmasking the contribution of low-level features to the guidance of attention. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3478-3487.

Otero, S. C., Weekes, B. S., & Hutton, S. B. (2011). Pupil size changes during recognition memory. Psychophysiology, 48, 1346-1353.

Otero-Millan, J. Macknik, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2012). Microsaccades and blinks trigger illusory rotation in the "Rotating Snakes" Illusion. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6043-6051.

Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S. L., Robbins, A., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2011). Stronger misdirection in curved than in straight motion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5:133. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00133.

Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S.L., Langston, R.E., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2013). An oculomotor continuum from exploration to fixation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 6175-6180

Otero-Millan, J., Schneider, R., Leigh, R.J., Macknik, S.L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2013). Saccades during attempted fixation in Parkinsonian Disorders and Recessive Ataxia: From microsaccades to square-wave jerks. PLoS ONE 8(3): e58535. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058535.

Otero-Millan, J., Serra, A., Leigh, R. J., Troncoso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2011). Distinctive features of saccadic intrusions and microsaccades in Progressive Supranuclear Palsy. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4379-4387.

Otero-Millan, J., Troncoso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., Serrano-Pedraza, I., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: Foundations for a common saccadic generator. Journal of Vision, 8(14):21, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/21/, doi:10.1167/8.14.21.

Óturai, G., Kolling, T., & Knopf, M. (2013). Relations between 18-month-olds’ gaze pattern and target action performance: A deferred imitation study with eye tracking. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 736-748.

Ouerhani, N., Bur, A., & Hügli, H. (2006). Linear vs. nonlinear feature combination for saliency computation: A comparison with human vision. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4174, 314-323.

Ouerhani, N., von Wartburg, R., Hugli, H., & Muri, R. (2004). Empirical validation of the saliency-based model of visual attention. Electronic Letters on Computer Vision and Image Analysis, 3, 13-24.

Over, E. A. B., Hooge, I. T. C., Vlaskamp, B. N. S., & Erkelens, C. J. (2007). Coarse-to-fine eye movement strategy in visual search. Vision Research, 47, 2272-2280.

Overvliet, K. E., Azañon, E., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2011). Somatosensory saccades reveal the timing of tactile spatial remapping. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3046-3052.

Özbek, M., & Bindemann, M. (2011). Exploring the time course of face matching: Temporal constraints impair unfamiliar face identification under temporally unconstrained viewing. Vision Research, 51, 2145-2155.

Back To Top

P

Pack, W., Carney, T., & Klein, S. A. (2013).Involuntary attention enhances identification accuracy for unmasked low contrast letters using non-predictive peripheral cues. Vision Research, 89, 79-89.

Pages, D.S., & Groh, J.M. (2013). Looking at the ventriloquist: Visual outcome of eye movements valibrates sound localization. PLoS ONE 8(8): e72562. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072562.

Pajak, M., & Nuthmann, A. (2013). Object-based saccadic selection during scene perception: Evidence from viewing position effects. Journal of Vision, 13(5):2, 1-21, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/5/2, doi:10.1167/13.5.2.

Palanica, A., & Itier, R. J. (2011). Searching for a perceived gaze direction using eye tracking. Journal of Vision, 11(2):19, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/2/19, doi:10.1167/11.2.19.

Palanica, A., & Itier, R. J. (2012). Attention capture by direct gaze is robust to context and task demands. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36, 123-134.

Palmer, J., & Moore, C. M. (2009). Using a filtering task to measure the spatial extent of selective attention. Vision Research, 49, 1045-1064.

Palmer, J., Huk, A. C., & Shadlen, M. N. (2005). The effect of stimulus strength on the speed and accuracy of a perceptual decision. Journal of Vision, 5, 376-404. http://journalofvision.org/5/5/1/

Palmer, S., & Mattler, U. (2013). Masked stimuli modulate endogenous shifts of spatial attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 486-503.

Palmer, S., & Mattler, U. (2013). On the source and scope of priming effects of masked stimuli on endogenous shifts of spatial attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 528-544.

Paltoglou, A. E., & Neri, P. (2012). Attentional control of sensory tuning in human visual perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 1260-1274.

Pan, J., Yan, M., Laubrock, J., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2013). Eye-voice span during rapid automatized naming of digits and dice in Chinese normal and dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 16, 967-979.

Pan, X., Sawa, K., Tsuda, I., Tsukada, M., & Sakagami, M. (2008). Reward prediction based on stimulus categorization in primate lateral prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 703-712.

Panagopoulos, A., von Grunau, M. W., & Galera, C. (2004). Attentive mechanisms in visual search. Spatial Vision, 17, 353-371.

Panizza, D., Chierchia, G., & Clifton, C. Jr. (2009). On the role of entailment patterns and scalar implicatures in the processing of numerals. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 503-518.

Pannasch, S., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2009). Distractor effect and saccade amplitudes: Further evidence on different modes of processing in free exploration of visual images. Visual Cognition, 17, 1109-1131.

Pannasch, S., Dornhoefer, S. M., Unema, P. J. A., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2001). The omnipresent prolongation of visual fixations: Saccades are inhibited by changes in situation and in subject's activity. Vision Research, 41, 3345-51.

Pannasch, S., Helmert, J. R., Müller, R., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2012). The analysis of eye movements in the context of cognitive technical systems: Three critical issues. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 7403, 19-34.

Pannasch, S., Helmert, J. R., Roth, K., Herbold, A. K., & Walter, H. (2008). Visual fixation durations and saccade amplitudes: Shifting relationship in a variety of conditions. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(2):4, 1-19.

Pannasch, S., Schulz, J., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2011). On the control of visual fixation durations in free viewing of complex images. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1120-1132.

Panouillères, M., Alahyane, N., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Nighoghossian, N., Gaymard, B., Tilikete, C., & Pélisson, D. (2013). Effects of structural and functional cerebellar lesions on sensorimotor adaptation of saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 231, 1-11.

Panouillères, M., Frismand, S., Sillan, O., Urquizar, C., Vighetto, A., Pélisson, D., & Tilikete, C. (2013). Saccades and eye-head coordination in ataxia with oculomotor apraxia type 2. The Cerebellum, 12, 557-567.

Panouillères, M., Gaveau, V., Socasau, C., Urquizar, C., & Pélisson, D. (2013). Brain processing of visual information during fast eye movements maintains motor performance. PLoS ONE 8(1): e54641. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054641.

Panouillères, M., Neggers, S. F. W., Gutteling, T. P., Salemme, R., van der Stigchel, S., van der Geest, J. N., Frens, M. A., & Pélisson, D. (2012). Transcranial magnetic stimulation and motor plasticity in human lateral cerebellum: Dual effect on saccadic adaptation. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 1512-1525.

Panouillères, M., Salemme, R., Urquizar, C. & Pélisson, D. (2012). Effect of saccadic adaptation on sequences of saccades. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(1): 1, 1-13.

Panouillères, M., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., & Pélisson, D. (2011). Sensory processing of motor inaccuracy depends on previously performed movement and on subsequent motor corrections: A study of the saccadic system. PLoS ONE 6(2): e17329. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017329

Panouillères, M., Weiss, T., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Munoz, D. P., & Pélisson, D. (2009). Behavioural evidence of separate adaptation mechanisms controlling saccade amplitude lengthening and shortening. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 1550-1559.

Paolozza, A., Rasmussen, C., Pei, J., Hanlon-Dearman, A., Nikkel, S. M., Andrew, G., McFarlane, A., Samdup, D., & Reynolds, J. N. (2014). Deficits in response inhibition correlate with oculomotor control in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder and prenatal alcohol exposure. Behavioural Brain Research, 259, 97-105.

Paolozza, A., Titman, R., Brien, D., Munoz, D. P., & Reynolds, J. N. (2013). Altered accuracy of saccadic eye movements in children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 37, 1491-1498.

Paquette, C., & Fung, J. (2007). Temporal facilitation of gaze in the presence of postural reactions triggered by sudden surface perturbations. Neuroscience, 145, 505-519.

Paquette, C., & Fung, J. (2011). Old age affects gaze and postural coordination. Gait & Posture, 33, 227-232.

Park, J., Schlag-Rey, M., & Schlag, J. (2003). Spatial localization precedes temporal determination in visual perception. Vision Research, 43, 1667-1674.

Park, J., Schlag-Rey, M., & Schlag, J. (2003). Voluntary action expands perceived duration of its sensory consequence. Experimental Brain Research, 149, 527-529.

Parton, A., Nachev, P., Hodgson, T. L., Mort, D., Thomas, D., Ordidge, R., Morgan, P. S., Jackson, S., Rees, G., & Husain, M. (2007). Role of the human supplementary eye field in the control of saccadic eye movements. Neuropsychologia, 45, 997-1008.

Pastukhov, A., & Braun, J. (2010). Rare but precious: Microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation. Vision Research, 50, 1173-1184.

Pastukhov, A., Vonau, V., Stonkute, S., & Braun, J. (2013). Spatial and temporal attention revealed by microsaccades. Vision Research, 85, 45-57.

Patalano, A. L., Juhasz, B. J., & Dicke, J. (2010). The relationship between indecisiveness and eye movement patterns in a decision making informational search task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23: 353-368.

Paterson, K. B., McGowan, V. A., & Jordan, T. R. (2012). Eye movements reveal effects of visual content on eye guidance and lexical access during reading. PLoS ONE 7(8): e41766. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041766.

Paterson, K. B., McGowan, V. A., & Jordan, T. R. (2013). Aging and the control of binocular fixations during reading. Psychology & Aging, 28, 789-795.

Paterson, K. B., McGowan, V. A., & Jordan, T. R. (2013). Filtered text reveals adult age differences in reading: Evidence from eye movements. Psychology & Aging, 28, 352-364.

Patsenko, E. G., & Altmann, E. M. (2010). How planful is routine behavior? A selective-attention model of performance in the Tower of Hanoi. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 95-116.

Patson, N. D., & Ferreira, F. (2009). Conceptual plural information is used to guide early parsing decisions: Evidence from garden-path sentences with reciprocal verbs. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 464-486.

Patson, N. D., & Warren, T. (2010). Eye movements when reading implausible sentences: Investigating potential structural influences on semantic integration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1516-1532.

Patson, N. D., & Warren, T. (2011). Building complex reference objects from dual sets. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 443-459.

Paubel, P.-V., Averty, P., & Raufaste, E. (2013). Effects of an automated conflict solver on the visual activity of air traffic controllers. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 23, 181-196.

Paulmann, S., Titone, D., & Pell, M. D. (2012). How emotional prosody guides your way: evidence from eye movements. Speech Communication, 54, 92-107.

Payne, B. R., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2012). Aging, parafoveal preview, and semantic integration in sentence processing: Testing the cognitive workload of wrap-up. Psychology and Aging, 27, 638-649.

Pearson, J. M., Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2010). Explicit information reduces discounting behavior in monkeys. Frontiers in Psychology, 1:237. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00237.

Pearson, J. M., Hayden, B. Y., Raghavachari, S., & Platt, M. L. (2009). Neurons in posterior cingulate cortex signal exploratory decisions in a dynamic multioption choice task. Current Biology, 19, 1532-1537.

Peck, C. J., Lau, B., & Salzman, C. D. (2013). The primate amygdala combines information about space and value. Nature Neuroscience 16, 340-348.

Peltsch, A., Hoffman, A., Armstrong, I., Pari, G., & Munoz, D. P. (2008). Saccadic impairments in Huntington's disease. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 457-469.

Perdreau, F., & Cavanagh, P. (2013). The artist’s advantage: Better integration of object information across eye movements. i-Perception, 4(6), 380-395.

Perea, M. (2012). Revisiting Huey: On the importance of the upper part of words during reading. Psychonomic Bulltin & Review, 19, 1148-1153.

Perea, M. (2013). Why does the APA recommend the use of serif fonts? Psicothema, 25, 13-17.

Perea, M., & Acha, J. (2009). Space information is important for reading. Vision Research, 49, 1994-2000.

Perea, M., & Gomez, P. (2012). Subtle increases in interletter spacing facilitate the encoding of words during normal reading. PLoS ONE 7(10): e47568. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047568.

Perea, M., Acha, J., & Carreiras, M. (2009). Eye movements when reading text messaging (txt msgng). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1560-1567.

Perea, M., Nakatani, C., & van Leeuwen, C. (2011). Transposition effects in reading Japanese Kana: Are they orthographic in nature? Memory & Cognition, 39, 700-707.

Perez, D. V., Lemoine, C., Siéroff, E., Ergis, A.-M., Bouhired, R., Rigault, E., & Doré-Mazars, K. (2012). The role of saccade preparation in lateralized word recognition: Evidence for the attentional bias theory. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2796-2804.

Perkins, A.M., Ettinger, U., Weaver, K., Schmechtig, A., Schrantee, A., Morrison, P. D., Sapara, A., Kumari, V., Williams, S. C., & Corr, P. J. (2013). Advancing the defensive explanation for anxiety disorders: lorazepam effects on human defense are systematically modulated by personality and threat-type. Translational Psychiatry (2013) 3, e246; doi:10.1038/tp.2013.

Perron, M., & Roy-Charland, A. (2013). Analysis of eye movements in the judgment of enjoyment and non-enjoyment smiles. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:659. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00659.

Perry, C. J., & Fallah, M. (2012). Color improves speed of processing but not perception in a motion illusion. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:92. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00092.

Pertzov, Y., Avidan, G., & Zohary, E. (2009). Accumulation of visual information across multiple fixations. Journal of Vision, 9(10):2, 1-12, http://journalofvision.org/9/10/2/, doi:10.1167/9.10.2.

Pertzov, Y., Dong, M. Y., Peich, M.-C., & Husain, M. (2012). Forgetting what was where: The fragility of object-location binding. PLoS ONE 7(10): e48214. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048214.

Pertzov, Y., Zohary, E., & Avidan, G. (2009). Implicitly perceived objects attract gaze during later free viewing. Journal of Vision, 9(6):6, 1-12, http://journalofvision.org/9/6/6/, doi:10.1167/9.6.6.

Pertzov, Y., Zohary, E., & Avidan, G. (2010). Rapid formation of spatiotopic representations as revealed by inhibition of return. Journal of Neurosciece, 30, 8882-8887.

Peschke, C., Hilgetag, C. C., & Olk, B. (2013). Influence of stimulus type on effects of flanker, flanker position, and trial sequence in a saccadic eye movement task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2253-2267.

Petersen, A., & Kyllingsbæk, S. (2013). Eye movements and practice effects in the attentional dwell time paradigm. Experimental Psychology, 60, 22-33.

Petersen, A., Kyllingsbæk, S., & Bundesen, C. (2012). Measuring and modeling attentional dwell time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1029-1046.

Petersen, A., Kyllingsbæk, S., & Bundesen, C. (2013). Attentional dwell times for targets and masks. Journal of Vision,13 (3):34, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/34, doi:10.1167/13.3.34.

Peterson, M. A., Cacciamani, L., Barense, M. D., & Scalf, P. E. (2012). The perirhinal cortex modulates V2 activity in response to the agreement between part familiarity and configuration familiarity. Hippocampus, 22, 1965-1977.

Peterson, M. F., & Eckstein, M. P. (2012). Looking just below the eyes is optimal across face recognition tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, E3314-E3323.

Peterson, M. F., & Eckstein, M. P. (2013). Individual differences in eye movements during face identification reflect observer-specific optimal points of fixation. Psychological Science, 24, 1216-1225.

Peterson, M. S., & Kramer, A. F. (2001). Attentional guidance of the eyes by contextual information and abrupt onsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 1239-1249.

Peterson, M. S., Beck, M. R., & Vomela, M. (2007). Visual search is guided by prospective and retrospective memory. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 123-135.

Peterson, M. S., Beck, M. R., & Wong, J. H. (2008). Were you paying attention to where you looked? The role of executive working memory in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 372-377.

Peterson, M. S., Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., & McCarley, J. S. (2004). Landmarks help guide attention during visual search. Spatial Vision, 17, 497-510.

Peterson, M. S., Kramer, A. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2004). Covert shifts of attention precede involuntary eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 398-405.

Peterson, M. S., Kramer, A. F., Irwin, D. E., & Hahn, S. (2002). Modulation of oculomotor control by abrupt onsets during attentionally demanding visual search. Visual Cognition, 9, 755-791.

Peterson, M. S., Kramer, A. F., Wang, F. R., Irwin, D. E., & McCarley, J. (2001). Visual search has memory. Psychological Science, 12, 287-292.

Peth, J., Kim, J. S. C., & Gamer, M. (2013). Fixations and eye-blinks allow for detecting concealed crime related memories. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88, 96-103.

Pfeffer, G., Abegg, M., Vertinsky, A. T., Ceccherini, I., Caroli, F., & Barton, J. J. (2011). The ocular motor features of adult-onset Alexander Disease: A case and review of the literature. Journal of Neuroophthalmology, 31, 155-159.

Pflugshaupt, T., Bopp, S. A., Heinemann, D., Mosimann, U. P., von Wartburg, R., Nyffeler, T., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2004). Residual oculomotor and exploratory deficits in patients with recovered hemineglect. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1203-1211.

Pflugshaupt, T., Gutbrod, K., Wurtz, P., von Wartburg, R., Nyffeler, T., de Haan, B., Karnath, H.-O., & Mueri, R. M. (2009). About the role of visual field defects in pure alexia. Brain, 132, 1907-1917.

Pflugshaupt, T., Mosimann, U. P., Schmitt, W. J., von Wartburg, R., Wurtz, P., Lüthi, M., Nyffeler, T., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2007). To look or not to look at threat?: Scanpath differences within a group of spider phobics. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 353-366.

Pflugshaupt, T., Mosimann, U. P., von Wartburg, R., Schmitt, W., Nyffeler, T., & Müri, R. M. (2005). Hypervigilance-avoidance pattern in spider phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 105-116.

Pflugshaupt, T., Nyffeler, T., von Wartburg, R., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2008). Loss of exploratory vertical saccades after unilateral frontal eye field damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 79, 474-477.

Pflugshaupt, T., Nyffeler, T., von Wartburg, R., Wurtz, P., Lüthi, M., Hubl, D., Gutbrod, K., Juengling, F. D., Hess, C. W., & Müri, R. M. (2007). When left becomes right and vice versa: Mirrored vision after cerebral hypoxia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2078-2091.

Pflugshaupt, T., Suchan, J., Mandler, M.-A., Sokolov, A. N., Trauzettel-Klosinski, S., Karnath, H.-O. (2011). Do patients with pure alexia suffer from a specific word form processing deficit? Evidence from "wrods with trasnpsoed letetrs". Neuropsychologia, 49, 1294-1301.

Pflugshaupt, T., von Wartburg, R., Wurtz, P., Chavesa, S., Déruaza, A., Nyffeler, T., von Arx, S., Lüthi, M., Cazzolia, D., & Müri, R. M. (2009). Linking physiology with behaviour: Functional specialisation of the visual field is reflected in gaze patterns during visual search. Vision Research, 49, 237-248.

Phillips, C. I., Sears, C. R., & Pexman, P. (2012). An embodied semantic processing effect on eye gaze during sentence reading. Language and Cognition, 4, 99-114.

Phillips, J. M., & Everling, S. (2012). Neural activity in the macaque putamen associated with saccades and behavioral outcome. PLoS ONE 7(12): e51596. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051596.

Phillips, M. H., & Edelman, J. A. (2008). The dependence of visual scanning performance on saccade, fixation, and perceptual metrics. Vision Research, 48, 926-936.

Pickering, M. J., McElree, B., Frisson, S., Chen., L., & Traxler, M. J. (2006). Underspecification and aspectual coercion. Discourse Processes, 42, 131-155.

Pinkhardt, E. H., & Kassubek, J. (2011). Ocular motor abnormalities in Parkinsonian syndromes. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 17, 223-230.

Pinkhardt, E. H., Jürgens, R., Becker, W., Valdarno, F., Ludolph, A. C., & Kassubek, J. (2008). Differential diagnostic value of eye movement recording in PSP-parkinsonism, Richardson's syndrome, and idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, 255, 1916-1925.

Pinkhardt, E. H., Jürgens, R., Lulé, D., Heimrath, J., & Ludolph, A. C., Becker, W., & Kassubek, J. (2012). Eye movement impairments in Parkinson's disease: possible role of extradopaminergic mechanisms. BMC Neurology, 12, 5. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-12-5.

Pinkhardt, E. H., Kassubek, J., Süssmuth, S., Ludolph, A. C., Becker, W., & Jürgens, R., (2009). Comparison of smooth pursuit eye movement deficits in multiple system atrophy and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, 256, 1438-1446.

Pisella, L., Alahyane, N., Blangero, A., Thery, F., Blanc, S., & Pelisson, D. (2011). Right-hemispheric dominance for visual remapping in humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 366, 572-585.

Pishyareh, E., Tehrani-Doost, M., Mahmoodi-Gharaie, J., Khorrami, A., Joudi, M., & Ahmadi, M. (2012). Attentional bias towards emotional scenes in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry, 7, 93-96.

Pivneva, I., Palmer, C., & Titone, D. (2012). Inhibitory control and l2 proficiency modulate bilingual language production: evidence from spontaneous monologue and dialogue speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:57.

Plainis, S., Petratou, D., Giannakopoulou, T., Radhakrishnan, H., Pallikaris, I. G., & Charman, N. W. (2013). Interocular differences in visual latency induced by reduced-aperture monovision. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 33, 123-129.

Plainis, S., Petratou, D., Giannakopoulou, T., Radhakrishnan, H., Pallikaris, I.G., Charman, W. N. (2013). Small-aperture monovision and the pulfrich experience: Absence of neural adaptation effects. PLoS ONE 8(10): e75987. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075987.

Plomp, G., Nakatani, C., Bonnardel, V., & van Leeuwen, C. (2004). Amodal completion as reflected by gaze durations. Perception, 33, 1185-1200.

Plummer, P., & Rayner, K. (2012). Effects of parafoveal word length and orthographic features on initial fixation landing positions in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 950-963.

Plummer, P., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2014). The influence of contextual diversity on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 275-283.

Plöchl, M., Ossandón, J. P., & König, P. (2012). Combining EEG and eye tracking: identification, characterization, and correction of eye movement artifacts in electroencephalographic data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6:278. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00278.

Poljac, E., & van den Berg, A. V. (2003). Representation of heading direction in far and near head space. Experimental Brain Research, 151, 501-513.

Poljac, E., & van den Berg, A. V. (2005). Localization of the plane of regard in space. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 457-467.

Poljac, E., Lankheet, M., & van den Berg, A. V. (2005). Perceptual compensation for eye torsion. Vision Research, 45, 485-496.

Poljac, E., Neggers, B., & van den Berg, A. V. (2006). Collision judgement of objects approaching the head. Experimental Brain Research, 171, 35-46.

Pollatsek, A., & Hyönä, J. (2005). The role of semantic transparency in the processing of Finnish compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 261-290.

Pollatsek, A., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2011). Processing novel and lexicalised Finnish compound words. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 795-810.

Pollatsek, A., Drieghe, D., Stockall, L., & de Almeida, R. G. (2010). The interpretation of ambiguous trimorphemic words in sentence context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 88-94.

Pollatsek, A., Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2000). The role of morphological constituents in reading Finnish compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 820-833.

Pollatsek, A., Slattery, T. J., & Juhasz, B. (2008). The processing of novel and lexicalised prefixed words in reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 1133-1158.

Pomplun, M. (2006). Saccadic selectivity in complex visual search displays. Vision Research, 46, 1886-1900.

Pomplun, M., Garaas, T., & Carrasco, M. (2013). The effects of task difficulty on visual search strategy in virtual 3D displays. Journal of Vision, 13(3):24, 1-22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/24, doi:10.1167/13.3.24.

Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2001). Investigating the visual span in comparative search: The effects of task difficulty and divided attention. Cognition, 81, B57-B67.

Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2001). The effects of peripheral and parafoveal cueing and masking on saccadic selectivity in a gaze-contingent window paradigm. Vision Research, 41, 2757-2769.

Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2003). Area activation: A computational model of saccadic selectivity in visual search. Cognitive Science, 27, 299-312.

Porras, G. C., & Guéhéneuc, Y.-G. (2010). An empirical study on the efficiency of different design pattern representations in UML class diagrams. Empirical Software Engineering, 15, 493-522.

Porter, G., Leonards, U., Wilcock, G., Haworth, J., Troscianko, T., & Tales, A. (2010). New insights into feature and conjunction search: II. Evidence from Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 46, 637-649.

Porter, G., Tales, A., & Leonards, U. (2010). What makes cast shadows hard to see? Journal of Vision, 10(3):13, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/10/3/13/, doi:10.1167/10.3.13.

Porter, G., Tales, A., Wilcock, G., Haworth, J., Troscianko, T., & Leonards, U. (2010). New insights into feature and conjunction search: I. Evidence from pupil size, eye movements and ageing. Cortex, 46, 621-636.

Porter, K. B., Caplovitz, G. P., Kohler, P. J., Ackerman, C. M., & Tse, P. U. (2011). Rotational and translational motion interact independently with form. Vision Research, 51, 2478-2487.

Porter, M. A., Shaw, T. A., & Marsh, P. J. (2010). An unusual attraction to the eyes in Williams-Beuren syndrome: A manipulation of facial affect while measuring face scanpaths. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 505-530.

Pradeep, A., Thomas, S., Roberts, E. O., Proudlock, F. A., & Gottlob, I. (2008). Reduction of congenital nystagmus in a patient after smoking cannabis. Strabismus, 16, 29-32.

Prado, C., Dubois, M., & Valdois, S. (2007). The eye movements of dyslexic children during reading and visual search: Impact of the visual attention span. Vision Research, 47, 2521-2530.

Pratt, J., & Neggers, B. (2008). Inhibition of return in single and dual tasks: Examining saccadic, keypress, and pointing responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 70, 257-265.

Pratt, J., & Trottier, L. (2005). Pro-saccades and anti-saccades to onset and offset targets. Vision Research, 45, 765-774.

Pratt, J., Dodd, M., & Welsh, T. (2006). Growing older does not always mean moving slower: Examining aging and the saccadic motor system. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 373-382.

Pratt, J., Shen, J., & Adam, J. J. (2004). The planning and execution of sequential eye movements: Saccades do not show the one target advantage. Human Movement Science, 22, 679-688.

Premereur, E., Vanduffel, W., & Janssen, P. (2011). Functional heterogeneity of Macaque lateral intraparietal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 12307-12317.

Premereur, E., Vanduffel, W., & Janssen, P. (2012). Local field potential activity associated with temporal expectations in the Macaque lateral intraparietal area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1314-1330.

Premereur, E., Vanduffel, W., Roelfsema, P. R., & Janssen, P. (2012). Frontal eye field microstimulation induces task-dependent gamma oscillations in the lateral intraparietal area. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 1392-1402.

Press, C. M., & Kilner, J. M. (2013). The time course of eye movements during action observation reflects sequence learning. Neuroreport, 24, 822-826.

Preston, T. J., Guo, F., Das, K., Giesbrecht, B., Eckstein, M. P. (2013). Neural representations of contextual guidance in visual search of real-world scenes. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 7846-7855.

Preuschoff, K., ’t Hart, B. M., & Einhäuser, W. (2011). Pupil dilation signals surprise: evidence for noradrenaline’s role in decision making. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5:115. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00115.

Price, J. M., & Sanford, A. J. (2012). Reading in healthy ageing: The influence of information structuring in sentences. Psychology and Aging, 27, 529-540.

Priess, H.-W., Born, S. & Ansorge, U. (2012). Inhibition of return after color singletons. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(5):2,1-12.

Primativo, S., Arduino, L. S., De Luca, M., Daini, R., & Martelli, M. (2013). Neglect dyslexia: A matter of "good looking". Neuropsychologia, 51, 2109-2119.

Prime, S. L., & Marotta, J. J. (2013). Gaze strategies during visually-guided versus memory-guided grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 291-305.

Prime, S. L., Vesia, M., & Crawford, J. D. (2010). TMS over human frontal eye fields disrupts trans-saccadic memory of multiple objects. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 759-772.

Privitera, C. M., Renninger, L. W., Carney, T., Klein, S., & Aguilar, M. (2010). Pupil dilation during visual target detection. Journal of Vision, 10(10):3, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/10/3, doi:10.1167/10.10.3.

Proudlock, F. A., & Gottlob, I. (2007). Physiology and pathology of eye-head coordination. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 26, 486-515.

Proudlock, F. A., Gottlob, I., & Constantinescu, C. S. (2002). Oscillopsia without nystagmus caused by head titubation in a patient with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology, 22, 88-91.

Proudlock, F. A., Khanna, A., & Gottlob, I. (2006). Filling-in along horizontal and vertical meridians. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 47, 453-460.

Proudlock, F. A., Shekhar, H., & Gottlob, I. (2003). Coordination of eye and head movements during reading. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 44, 2991-2998.

Proudlock, F. A., Shekhar, H., & Gottlob, I. (2004). Age-related changes in head and eye coordination. Neurobiology of Aging, 25, 1377-1385.

Puig, M. V., & Miller, E. K. (2012). The role of prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors in the neural mechanisms of associative learning. Neuron, 74, 874-886.

Purcell, B. A., Weigand, P. K., Schall, J. D. (2012). Supplementary eye field during visual search: Salience, cognitive control, and performance monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 10273-10285.

Pyykkönen, P., & Järvikivi, J. (2010). Activation and persistence of implicit causality information in spoken language comprehension. Experimental Psychology, 57, 5-16.

Pyykkönen, P., Hyönä, J., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2010). Activating gender stereotypes during online spoken language processing: Evidence from visual world eye-tracking. Experimental Psychology, 57, 126-133.

Pérez, J. A., & Passini, S. (2012). Avoiding minorities: Social invisibility. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 864-874.

Back To Top

Q

Qian, M., Aguilar, M., Zachery, K. N., Privitera, C., Klein, S., Carney, T., & Nolte, L. W. (2009). Decision-level fusion of EEG and pupil features for single-trial visual detection analysis. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 56, 1929-1937.

Quigley, C., Onat, S., Harding, S., Cooke, M., & König, P. (2008). Audio-visual integration during overt visual attention. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(2):4, 1-17.

Quigley, L., Nelson, A. L., Carriere, J., Smilek, D., & Purdon, C. (2012). The effects of trait and state anxiety on attention to emotional images: An eye-tracking study. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 1390-1411.

Quinlan, D. J., & Culham, J. C. (2007). fMRI reveals a preference for near viewing in the human parieto-occipital cortex. NeuroImage, 36, 167-187.

Quiroga, R. Q., & Pedreira, C. (2011). How do we see art: an eye-tracker study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5:98. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00098.

Back To Top

R

Rach, S., & Diederich, A. (2006). Visual-tactile integration: does stimulus duration influence the relative amount of response enhancement? Experimental Brain Research, 173, 514-520.

Rach, S., Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2011). On quantifying multisensory interaction effects in reaction time and detection rate. Psychological Research, 75, 77-94.

Radach, R., Huestegge, L., & Reilly, R. (2008). The role of global top-down factors in local eye-movement control in reading. Psychological Research, 72, 675-688.

Radach, R., Inhoff, A. W., Glover, L., & Vorstius, C. (2013). Contextual constraint and N+2 preview effects in reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 619-633.

Radach, R., Inhoff, A., & Heller, D. (2004). Orthographic regularity gradually modulates saccade amplitudes in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 27-51.

Radach, R., Lemmer, S., Vorstius, C., Heller, D., & Radach, K. (2003). Eye movements in the processing of print advertisements. In J. Hyönä, R. Radach & H. Deubel (Eds.), The Mind's Eyes: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research (pp. 609-632). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Raemaekers, M., Bergsma, D. P., van Wezel, R. J. A., van der Wildt, G. J., & van den Berg, A. V. (2011). Effects of vision restoration training on early visual cortex in patients with cerebral blindness investigated with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 872 - 882.

Rahman, W., Proudlock, F., & Gottlob, I. (2006). Oral gabapentin treatment for symptomatic Heimann-Bielschowsky phenomenon. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 141, 221-222.

Raimondo, F., Kamienkowski, J. E., Sigman, M., & Slezak, D. F. (2012). CUDAICA: GPU Optimization of Infomax-ICA EEG Analysis, Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, vol. 2012, Article ID 206972, 8 pages, 2012. doi:10.1155/2012/206972.

Ramkumar, P., Jas, M., Pannasch, S., Hari, R., & Parkkonen, L. (2013). Feature-specific information processing precedes concerted activation in human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 7691-7699.

Rappaport, S. J., Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2013). The attraction of yellow corn: Reduced attentional constraints on coding learned conjunctive relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1016-1031.

Rasche, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. (2010). Orienting during gaze guidance in a letter-Identification task. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(4):3, 1-10.

Rasche, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Visual orienting in dynamic broadband (1/f) noise sequences. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 100-113.

Rastgardani, T., Lau, V., Barton, J. J. S., & Abegg, M. (2012). Trial history biases the spatial programming of antisaccades. Experimental Brain Rresearch, 222, 175-183.

Rayner, K., & Johnson, R. L. (2005). Letter-by-letter acquired dyslexia is due to the serial encoding of letters. Psychological Science, 16, 530-534.

Rayner, K., Angele, B., Schotter, E. R., & Bicknell, K. (2013). On the processing of canonical word order during eye fixations in reading: Do readers process transposed word previews? Visual Cognition, 21, 353-381.

Rayner, K., Castelhano, M. S., & Yang, J. (2009). Eye movements and the perceptual span in older and younger readers. Psychology and Aging, 24, 755-760.

Rayner, K., Castelhano, M. S., & Yang, J. (2009). Eye movements when looking at unusual/weird scenes: Are there cultural differences? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 254-259.

Rayner, K., Castelhano, M. S., & Yang, J. (2010). Preview benefit during eye fixations in reading for older and younger readers. Psychology and Aging, 25, 714-718.

Rayner, K., Chace, K. H., Slattery, T. J., & Ashby, J. (2006). Eye movements as reflections of comprehension processes in reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 241-255.

Rayner, K., Li, X., Juhasz, B. J., & Yan, G. (2005). The effect of word predictability on the eye movements of Chinese readers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1089-1093.

Rayner, K., Li, X., Williams, C. C., Cave, K. R., & Well, A. D. (2007). Eye movements during information processing tasks: Individual differences and cultural effects. Vision Research, 47, 2714-2726.

Rayner, K., Miller, B., & Rotello, C. M. (2008). Eye movements when looking at print advertisements: the goal of the viewer matters. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 697-707.

Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D., Stroud, M. J., Williams, C. C., & Pollatsek, A. (2006). The Effect of word frequency, word predictability, and font difficulty on the eye movements of young and older readers. Psychology & Aging, 21, 448-465.

Rayner, K., Rotello, C. M., Stewart, A. J., Keir, J., & Duffy, S. A. (2001). Integrating text and pictorial information: Eye movements when looking at print advertisements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 219-226.

Rayner, K., Slattery, T, J., Drieghe, D., & Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Eye movements and word skipping during reading: Effects of word length and predictability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 514-528.

Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., & Bélanger, N. N. (2010). Eye movements, the perceptual span, and reading speed. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 834-839.

Rayner, K., Smith, T. J., Malcolm, G. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Eye movements and visual encoding during scene perception. Psychological Science, 20, 6-10.

Rayner, K., Yang, J., Castelhano, M. S., & Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading disappearing text. Psychology and Aging, 26, 214-223.

Rayner, K., Yang, J., Schuett, S., & Slattery, T. (2013). Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading unspaced text. Experimental Psychology, 60, 354-361.

Rees, A. L., Kabanarou, S. A., Culham, L. E., & Rubin, G. S. (2005). Can retinal eccentricity predict visual acuity and contrast sensitivity at the PRL in AMD patients? International Congress Series, 1282, 694-698.

Reeve, P., Clark, J. J., & O'Regan, J. K. (2008). Convergent flash localization near saccades without equivalent "compression" of perceived separation. Journal of Vision, 8(13):5, 1-19, http://journalofvision.org/8/13/5/, doi:10.1167/8.13.5.

Rehder, B., & Hoffman, A. B. (2005). Eyetracking and selective attention in category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 1-41.

Rehder, B., & Hoffman, A. B. (2005). Thirty-something categorization results explained: Selective attention, eyetracking, and models of category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 811-829.

Rehder, B., Colner, R. M., & Hoffman, A. B. (2009). Feature inference learning and eyetracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 393-419.

Reichelt, B., Kühnel, S., & Dal Mas, D. E. (2013). The influence of explicit and implicit memory processes on experience-dependent eye movements. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 82, 455-460.

Reichle, E. D., Reineberg, A. E., & Schooler, J. W. (2010). Eye movements during mindless reading. Psychological Science, 21, 1300-1310.

Reichle, E. D., Vanyukov, P. M., Laurent, P. A., & Warren, T. (2008). Serial or parallel? Using depth-of-processing to examine attention allocation during reading. Vision Research, 48, 1831-1836.

Reilly, R., & Radach, R. (2006). Some empirical tests of an interactive activation model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 34-55.

Reingold, E. M, Yang, J., & Rayner, K. (2010). The time course of word frequency and case alternation effects on fixation times in reading: Evidence for lexical control of eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1677-1683.

Reingold, E. M. (2002). On the perceptual specificity of memory representations. Memory, 10, 365-379.

Reingold, E. M., & Loschky, L. C. (2002). Saliency of peripheral targets in gaze-contingent multiresolutional displays. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 34, 491-499.

Reingold, E. M., & Rayner, K. (2006). Examining the word identification stages hypothesized by the E-Z reader model. Psychological Science, 17, 742-746.

Reingold, E. M., & Stampe, D. M. (2000). Saccadic inhibition and gaze contingent research paradigms. In Kennedy, Alan, Radach, Ralph et al. (Eds.) Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 119-145). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers.

Reingold, E. M., & Stampe, D. M. (2002). Saccadic inhibition in voluntary and reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 371-388.

Reingold, E. M., & Stampe, D. M. (2004). Saccadic inhibition in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 194-211.

Reingold, E. M., Charness, N., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). Visual span in expert chess players: Evidence from eye movements. Psychological Science, 12, 48-55.

Reingold, E. M., Reichle, E. D., Glaholt, M. G., & Sheridan, H. (2012). Direct lexical control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Cognitive Psychology, 65, 177-206.

Reinhart, R. M. G., Heitz, R. P., Purcell, B. A., Weigand, P. K., Schall, J. D., & Woodman, G. F. (2012). Homologous mechanisms of visuospatial working memory maintenance in macaque and human: Properties and sources. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 7711-7722.

Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., Mogg, K., Benson, V., Bradley, B. P., Hardin, M. G., Liversedge, S. P., Pine, D. S., & Ernst, M. (2012). Anxiety and selective attention to angry faces: An antisaccade study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 54-65.

Reinisch, E., & Sjerps, M. J. (2013). The uptake of spectral and temporal cues in vowel perception is rapidly influenced by context. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 101-116.

Reinisch, E., & Weber, A. (2012). Adapting to suprasegmental lexical stress errors in foreign-accented speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132, 1165-1176.

Reinisch, E., Jesse, A., & McQueen, J. M. (2010). Early use of phonetic information in spoken word recognition: Lexical stress drives eye movements immediately. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 772-783.

Reinisch, E., Jesse, A., & McQueen, J. M. (2011). Speaking rate from proximal and distal contexts is used during word segmentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 978-996.

Ren, G.-Q., & Yang, Y. (2010). Syntactic boundaries and comma placement during silent reading of Chinese text: evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 168-177.

Ren, Y., Xuan, Y., & Fu, X. (2007). Effects of pattern complexity on information integration: Evidence from eye movements. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4562, 582-590.

Reschke, M., Somers, J. T., Leigh, R. J., Krnavek, J. M., Kornilova, L., Kozlovskaya, I., Bloomberg, J. J., & Paloski, W. H. (2004). Sensorimotor recovery following spaceflight may be due to frequent square-wave saccadic intrusions. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 75, 700-704.

Reuter, B., Möllers, D., Bender, J., Schwehn, A., Ziemek, J., Gallinat, J., & Kathman, N. (2011). Volitional saccades and attentional mechanisms in schizophrenia patients and healthy control subjects. Psychophysiology, 48, 1333-1339.

Revill, K. P., & Spieler, D. H. (2012). The effect of lexical frequency on spoken word recognition in young and older listeners. Psychology and Aging, 27, 80-87.

Revill, K. P., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2008). Context and spoken word recognition in a novel lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 1207-1223.

Rey, F., Lê, T.-T., Bertin, R., & Kapoula, Z. (2008). Saccades horizontal or vertical at near or at far do not deteriorate postural control. Auris Nasus Larynx, 35, 185-191.

Reynolds, M. G., Eastwood, J. D., Partanen, M., Frischen, A., & Smilek, D. (2009). Monitoring eye movements while searching for affective faces. Visual Cognition, 17, 318-333.

Ricciardelli, P., Betta, E., Pruner, S., & Turatto, M. (2009). Is there a direct link between gaze perception and joint attention behaviours? Effects of gaze contrast polarity on oculomotor behaviour. Experimental Brain Research, 194, 347-357.

Ricciardelli, P., Bricolo, E., Aglioti, S. M., & Chelazzi, L. (2002). My eyes want to look where your eyes are looking: Exploring the tendency to imitate another individual's gaze. NeuroReport, 13, 2259-2264.

Richards, H. J., Benson, V., & Hadwin, J. A. (2012). The attentional processes underlying impaired inhibition of threat in anxiety: The remote distractor effect. Cognition and Emotion, 934-942.

Richards, H. J., Hadwin, J. A., Benson, V., Wenger, M. J., & Donnelly, N. (2011). The influence of anxiety on processing capacity for threat detection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 883-889.

Richardson, B. A., & Balasubramaniam, R. (2010). The effect of entrainment on the timing of periodic eye movements. Neuroscience Letters, 469, 117-121.

Richardson, B. A., Cluff, T., Lyons, J., & Balasubramaniam, R. (2013). An eye-to-hand magnet effect reveals distinct spatial interference in motor planning and execution. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 443-454.

Richardson, B. A., Ratneswaran, A., Lyons, J., & Balasubramaniam, R. (2011). The time course of online trajectory corrections in memory-guided saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 457-469.

Richlan, F., Gagl, B., Schuster, S., Hawelka, S., Humenberger, J., & Hutzler, F. (2013). A new high-speed visual stimulation method for gaze-contingent eye movement and brain activity studies. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7:24. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00024.

Richstone, L., Schwartz, M. J., Seideman, C., Cadeddu, J., Marshall, S., Kavoussi, Louis, R, (2010). Eye metrics as an objective assessment of surgical skill. Annals of Surgery, 252, 177-182.

Riecanský, I., Thiele, A., Distler, C., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (2005). Chromatic sensitivity of neurones in area MT of the anaesthetised macaque monkey compared to human motion perception. Experimental Brain Research, 167, 504-525.

Rieger, G., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2012). The eyes have it: Sex and sexual orientation differences in pupil dilation patterns. PLoS ONE 7(8): e40256. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040256.

Rieger, G., Rosenthal, A. M., Cash, B. M., Linsenmeier, J. A. W., Bailey, J. M., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2013). Male bisexual arousal: A matter of curiosity? Biological Psychology, 94, 479-489.

Rieiro, H., Martinez-Conde, S., Danielson, A. P., Pardo-Vazquez, J. L., Srivastava, N., & Macknik, S. L. (2012). Optimizing the temporal dynamics of light to human perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 19828-19833.

Rienhoff, R., Baker, J., Fischer, L., Strauß, B., & Schorer, J. (2012). Field of vision influences sensory-motor control of skilled and less-skilled dart players. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, 11, 542-550.

Riggs, L., McQuiggan, D. A., Farb, N., Anderson, A. K., & Ryan, J. D. (2011). The role of overt attention in emotion-modulated memory. Emotion, 11, 776-785.

Riggs, L., Mcquiggan, D. A., Anderson, A. K., & Ryan, J. D. (2010). Eye movement monitoring reveals differential influences of emotion on memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 1:205. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00205.

Rigoulot, S., & Pell, M. D. (2012). Seeing emotion with your ears: Emotional prosody implicitly guides visual attention to faces. PLoS ONE 7(1): e30740. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030740.

Rijkaart, D. C., van der Geest, J. N., Kelders, W. P., de Zeeuw, C. I., & Frens, M. A. (2004). Short-term adaptation of the cervico-ocular reflex. Experimental Brain Research, 156, 124-128.

Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2006). Spider fearful individuals attend to threat, then quickly avoid it: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 231-238.

Rinck, M., Gámez, E., Díaz, J. M., & de Vega, M. (2003). Processing of temporal information: Evidence from eye movements. Memory & Cognition, 31, 77-86.

Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 235-248.

Rishel, C. A., Huang, G., & Freedman, D. J. (2013). Independent category and spatial encoding in parietal Cortex. Neuron, 77, 969-979.

Risko, E. F., Anderson, N. C., Lanthier, S., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Curious eyes: Individual differences in personality predict eye movement behavior in scene-viewing. Cognition, 122, 86-90.

Risko, E. F., Maloney, E. A., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2013). Paying attention to attention: evidence for an attentional contribution to the size congruity effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1137-1147.

Risse, S., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Adult age differences in the perceptual span during reading. Psychology and Aging, 26, 451-460.

Risse, S., & Kliegl, R. (2012). Evidence for delayed parafoveal-on-foveal effects from word n+2 in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1026-1042.

Ritchie, A. E., Griffiths, P. G., Chinnery, P. F., & Davidson, A. W. (2010). Eye movement recordings to investigate a supranuclear component in chronic progressive external ophthalmoplegia: a cross-sectional study. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 94, 1165-1168.

Ritchie, K. L., Hunt, A. R., & Sahrai, A. (2012). Trans-saccadic priming in hemianopia: Sighted-field sensitivity is boosted by a blind-field prime. Neuropsychologia, 50, 997-1005.

Roberts, E. O., Proudlock, F. A., Martin, K., Reveley, M. A., Al-Uzri, M., & Gottlob, I. (2013). Reading in schizophrenic subjects and their nonsymptomatic first-degree relatives. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39, 896-907.

Roberts, J.A., Wallis, G., & Breakspear, M. (2013). Fixational eye movements during viewing of dynamic natural scenes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:797. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00797.

Roberts, L., Gullberg, M., & Indefrey, P. (2008). Online pronoun resolution in L2 discourse: L1 influence and general learner effects. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 333-357.

Roberts, L., Matsuo, A., & Duffield, N. (2013). Processing VP-ellipsis and VP-anaphora with structurally parallel and nonparallel antecedents: An eye-tracking study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 29-47.

Rodger, H., Kelly, D. J., Blais, C., & Caldara, R. (2010). Inverting faces does not abolish cultural diversity in eye movements. Perception, 39, 1491-1503.

Roefs, A., Jansen, A., Moresi, S., Willems, P., van Grootel, S., & van der Borgh, A. (2008). Looking good: BMI, attractiveness bias and visual attention. Appetite, 51, 552-555.

Roelfsema, P. R., Houtkamp, R., & Korjoukov, I. (2010). Further evidence for the spread of attention during contour grouping: A reply to Crundall, Dewhurst, and Underwood (2008). Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 849-862.

Roelofs, A. (2007). Attention and gaze control in picture naming, word reading, and word categorizing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 232-251.

Roelofs, A. (2008). Attention, gaze shifting, and dual-task interference from phonological encoding in spoken word planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1580-1598.

Roelofs, A. (2008). Tracing attention and the activation flow in spoken word planning using eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 353-368.

Roelofs, A. (2011). Attention, exposure duration, and gaze shifting in naming performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 860-873.

Rohs, M., Schleicher, R., Schöning, J., Essl, G., Naumann, A., & Krüger, A. (2009). Impact of item density on the utility of visual context in magic lens interactions. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 13, :633-646.

Roijendijk, L., Farquhar, J., van Gerven, M., Jensen, O., & Gielen, S. (2013). Exploring the impact of target eccentricity and task difficulty on covert visual spatial attention and its implications for brain computer interfacing. PLoS ONE 8(12): e80489. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080489.

Rolfs, M., & Carrasco, M. (2012). Rapid simultaneous enhancement of visual sensitivity and perceived contrast during saccade preparation. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 13744-13752.

Rolfs, M., & Vitu, F. (2007). On the limited role of target onset in the gap task: Support for the motor-preparation hypothesis. Journal of Vision, 7(10):7, 1-20, http://journalofvision.org/7/10/7/, doi:10.1167/7.10.7.

Rolfs, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2004). Microsaccade orientation supports attentional enhancement opposite to a peripheral cue. Psychological Science, 15, 705-707.

Rolfs, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Crossmodal coupling of oculomotor control and spatial attention in vision and audition. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 427-439.

Rolfs, M., Jonikaitis, D., Deubel, H., & Cavanagh, P. (2011). Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 252-256.

Rolfs, M., Kliegl, R., & Engbert, R. (2008). Toward a model of microsaccade generation: The case of microsaccadic inhibition. Journal of Vision, 8(11):5, 1-23, http://journalofvision.org/8/11/5/, doi:10.1167/8.11.5.

Rolfs, M., Knapen, T., & Cavanagh, P. (2010). Global saccadic adaptation. Vision Research, 50, 1882-1890.

Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2006). Shortening and prolongation of saccade latencies following microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 369-376.

Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Microsaccade- induced prolongation of saccadic latencies depends on microsaccade amplitude. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(3):1, 1-8.

Rolheiser, T. M., Binsted, G., & Brownell, K. J. (2006). Visuomotor representation decay: influence on motor systems. Experimental Brain Research, 173, 698-707.

Romano, M. C., Thiel, M., Kurths, J., Mergenthaler, K., & Engbert, R. (2009). Hypothesis test for synchronisation: Twin surrogates revisited. Chaos, 19, 015108: 1-14.

Romero, M. C., Van Dromme, I., & Janssen, P. (2012). Responses to two-dimensional shapes in the macaque anterior intraparietal area. European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2324-2334.

Romero, M.C., Van Dromme, I.C.L., & Janssen, P. (2013). The role of binocular disparity in stereoscopic images of objects in the macaque anterior intraparietal area. PLoS ONE 8(2): e55340. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055340.

Rommelse, N. N. J., Van der Stigchel, S., Witlox, J., Geldof, C., Deijen, J.-B. , Theeuwes, J., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Deficits in visuo-spatial working memory, inhibition and oculomotor control in boys with ADHD and their non-affected brothers. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 249-260.

Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2013). The contents of predictions in sentence comprehension: Activation of the shape of objects before they are referred to. Neuropsychologia, 51, 437-447.

Rosenthal, C. R., Roche-Kelly, E. E., Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (2009). Response-dependent contributions of human primary motor cortex and angular gyrus to manual and perceptual sequence learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 15115 - 15125.

Rosenthal, C.R., Ng, T.W.C., & Kennard, C. (2013). Generalisation of new sequence knowledge depends on response modality. PLoS ONE 8(2): e53990. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053990.

Ross, M., Lanyon, L. J., Viswanathan, J., Manoach, D. S., & Barton, J. J. S. (2011). Human prosaccades and antisaccades under risk: Effects of penalties and rewards on visual selection and the value of actions. Neuroscience, 196, 168-177.

Ross, N. M., & Kowler, E. (2013). Eye movements while viewing narrated, captioned, and silent videos. Journal of Vision, 13(4):1, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/4/1, doi:10.1167/13.4.1.

Rossit, S., Szymanek, L., Butler, S. H., & Harvey, M. (2010). Memory-guided saccade processing in visual form agnosia (patient DF). Experimental Brain Research, 200, 109-116.

Roth, T., Sokolov, A., N., Messias, A., Roth, P., Weller, M., & Trauzettel-Klosinski, S. (2009). Comparing explorative saccade and flicker training in hemianopia: A randomized controlled study. Neurology, 72, 324-331.

Rotman, G., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. (2004). Mislocalization of targets flashed during smooth pursuit depends on the change in gaze direction after the flash. Journal of Vision. 4, 564-57. http://journalofvision.org/4/7/4/.

Rotman, G., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. (2005). Flashes are localized as if they were moving with the eyes. Vision Research, 45, 355-364.

Roux, P., Passerieux, C., & Ramus, F. (2013). Kinematics matters: A new eye-tracking investigation of animated triangles. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 229-244.

Roy-Charland, A., Saint-Aubin, J., & Evans, M. A. (2007). Eye movements in shared book reading with children from kindergarten to Grade 4. Reading and Writing, 20, 909-931.

Roy-Charland, A., Saint-Aubin, J., Klein, R. M., & Lawrence, M. (2007). Eye movements as direct tests of the GO model for the missing-letter effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 324-337.

Roy-Charland, A., Saint-Aubin, J., Klein, R. M., MacLean, G. H., Lalande, A., & Bélanger, A. (2012). Eye movements when reading: The importance of the word to the left of fixation. Visual Cognition, 20, 328-355.

Roy-Charland, A., Saint-Aubin, J., Lawrence, M. A., & Klein, R. M. (2009). Solving the chicken and egg problem of letter detection and fixation duration in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1553-1562.

Rubin, G. S., & Feely, M. (2009). The role of eye movements during reading in patients with age-related macular degeneration (AMD). Neuro-Ophthalmology, 33, 120-126.

Rubin, R. D., Brown-Schmidt, S., Duff, M. C., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2011). How do I remember that I know you know that I know? Psychological Science, 22, 1574 - 1582.

Ruiz, O., & Paradiso, M. A. (2012). Macaque V1 representations in natural and reduced visual contexts: Spatial and temporal properties and the influence of saccadic eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108, 324-333.

Rupp, J., Blekher, T., Jackson, J., Beristain, X., Marshall, J., Hui, S., Wojcieszek, J., & Foroud, T.(2010). Progression in prediagnostic Huntington disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 81, 379-384.

Rupp, J., Dzemidzic, M., Blekher, T., West, J., Hui, S., Wojcieszek, J., Saykin, A. J., Kareken, D. A., & Foroud, T. (2012). Comparison of vertical and horizontal saccade measures and their relation to gray matter changes in premanifest and manifest Huntington disease. Journal of Neurology, 259, 267-276.

Russell, T. A., Green, M. J., Simpson, I., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Remediation of facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: Concomitant changes in visual attention. Schizophrenia Research, 103, 248-256.

Rutishauser, U., & Koch, C. (2007). Probabilistic modeling of eye movement data during conjunction search via feature-based attention. Journal of Vision, 7(6):5, 1-20, http://journalofvision.org/7/6/5/, doi:10.1167/7.6.5.

Ryan, J. D., & Villate, C. (2009). Building visual representations: The binding of relative spatial relations across time. Visual Cognition, 17, 254-272.

Ryan, J. D., Leung, G., Turk-Browne, N. B., & Hasher, L. (2007). Assessment of age-related changes in inhibition and binding using eye movement monitoring. Psychology & Aging, 22, 239-250.

Ryan, J. D., Riggs, L., & McQuiggan, D. A. (2010). Eye movement monitoring of memory. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 42, https://www.jove.com/details.stp?id=2108, doi: 10.3791/2108.

Ryan, J. D., Shen, J., & Reingold, E. M. (2006). Modulation of distraction in ageing. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 339-351.

Rycroft, N., Hutton, S. B., & Rusted, J. M. (2006). The antisaccade task as an index of sustained goal activation in working memory: modulation by nicotine. Psychopharmacology, 188, 521-529.

Rycroft, N., Hutton, S. B., Clowry, O., Groomsbridge, C., Sierakowski, A., & Rusted, J. M. (2007). Non-cholinergic modulation of antisaccade performance: a modafinil-nicotine comparison. Psychopharmacology, 195, 245-253.

Rycroft, N., Rusted, J. M., & Hutton, S. B. (2005). Acute effects of nicotine on visual search tasks in young adult smokers. Psychopharmacology, 181, 160-169.

Ryu, D., Abernethy, B., Mann, D. L., Poolton, J. M., & Gorman, A. D. (2013). The role of central and peripheral vision in expert decision making. Perception, 42, 591-607.

Rämä, P., & Baccino, T. (2010). Eye fixation-related potentials (EFRPs) during object identification. Visual Neuroscience, 27, 187-192.

Back To Top

S

Sachs, A. J., Khayat, P. S., Niebergall, R., & Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2011). A metric-based analysis of the contribution of spike timing to contrast and motion direction coding by single neurons in macaque area MT. Brain Research, 1368, 163-184.

Sadaghiani, S., Maier, J. X., & Noppeney, U. (2009). Natural, metaphoric, and linguistic auditory direction signals have distinct influences on visual motion processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 6490-6499.

Sadeghi, N. G., Pariyadath, V., Apte, S., Eagleman, D. M., & Cook, E. P. (2011). Neural correlates of subsecond time distortion in the middle temporal area of visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3829-3840.

Sagarra, N., & Ellis, N. C. (2013). From seeing adverbs to seeing verbal morphology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 261-290.

Saijo, N., Murakami, I., Nishida, S., & Gomi, H. (2005). Large-field visual motion directly induces an involuntary rapid manual following response. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4941-4951.

Sainio, M., Hyönä, J., Bingushi, K., & Bertram, R. (2007). The role of interword spacing in reading Japanese: An eye movement study. Vision Research, 47, 2575-2584.

Saint-Aubin, J. (2010). De l’impossibilité d’un traitement syntaxique parafovéal à longue portée : une réplique à Foucambert (2008). Psychologie Française, 55, 211-222.

Saint-Aubin, J., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Influence of parafoveal processing on the missing-letter effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 318-334.

Saint-Aubin, J., Kenny, S., & Roy-Charland, A. (2010). The role of eye movements in the missing-letter effect revisited with the rapid serial visual presentation procedure. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 47-52.

Saint-Aubin, J., Tremblay, S., & Jalbert, A. (2007). Eye movements and serial memory for visual-spatial information: Does time spent fixating contribute to recall? Experimental Psychology, 54, 264-272.

Sajin, S. M., & Connine, C. M. (2014). Semantic richness: The role of semantic features in processing spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 13-35.

Sakaguchi, Y. (2003). Visual field anisotropy revealed by perceptual filling-in. Vision Research, 43, 2029-2038.

Salinas, E., Shankar, S., Costello, M. G., Zhu, D., & Stanford, T. R. (2010). Waiting is the hardest part: comparison of two computational strategies for performing a compelled-response task. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 4:153. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00153.

Salmerón, L., Baccino, T., Cañas, J. J., Madrid, R. I., & Fajardo, I. (2009). Do graphical overviews facilitate or hinder comprehension in hypertext? Computers & Education, 53, 1308-1319.

Salverda, A. P., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2011). Attentional capture of objects referred to by spoken language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1122-1133.

Salverda, A. P., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2010). Tracking the time course of orthographic information in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1108-1117.

Salverda, A. P., Dahan, D., & McQueen, J. M. (2003). The role of prosodic boundaries in the resolution of lexical embedding in speech comprehension. Cognition, 90, 51-89.

Salverda, A. P., Kleinschmidt, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2014). Immediate effects of anticipatory coarticulation in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 145-163.

Sanchez, C. A., & Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 34, 344-355.

Sander, V., Soper, B., & Everling, S. (2010). Nonhuman primate event-related potentials associated with pro- and anti-saccades. NeuroImage, 49, 1650-1658.

Sapkota, R. P., Pardhan, S., & van der Linde, I. (2011). The impact of extrafoveal information on visual short-term memory for object position. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 574-585.

Sarvananthan, N., Proudlock, F. A., Choudhuri, I., Dua, H., & Gottlob, I. (2006). Pharmacologic treatment of congenital nystagmus. Archives of Ophthalmology, 124, 916-918.

Sarès, F., Granjon, L., Benraiss, A., & Boulinguez, P. (2007). Analyzing head roll and eye torsion by means of offline image processing. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 590-599.

Satel, J., & Wang, Z. (2012). Investigating a two causes theory of inhibition of return. Experimental Brain Research, 223, 469-478.

Satel, J., Hilchey, M. D., Wang, Z., Story, R., & Klein, R. M. (2013). The effects of ignored versus foveated cues upon inhibition of return: An event-related potential study. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 29-40.

Sato, N., & Yamaguchi, Y. (2007). Theta synchronization networks emerge during human object-place memory encoding. NeuroReport, 18, 419-424.

Sato, N., & Yamaguchi, Y. (2009). A computational predictor of human episodic memory based on a theta phase precession network. PLoS ONE 4(10): e7536. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007536.

Sauleau, P., Pollak, P., Krack, P., Courjon, J.-H., Vighetto, A., Benabid, A.-L., Pélisson, D., & Tilikete, C. (2008). Subthalamic stimulation improves orienting gaze movements in Parkinson's disease. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 1857-1863.

Sauleau, P., Pollak, P., Krack, P., Pélisson, D., Vighetto, A., Benabid, A.-L., & Tilikete, C. (2007). Contraversive eye deviation during stimulation of the subthalamic region. Movement Disorders, 22, 1810 - 1813.

Saunders, D. R., Williamson, D. K., & Troje, N. F. (2010). Gaze patterns during perception of direction and gender from biological motion. Journal of Vision, 10(11):9, 1-10, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/11/9, doi:10.1167/10.11.9.

Savage, S. W., Potter, D. D., & Tatler, B. W. (2012). Does preoccupation impair hazard perception? A simultaneous EEG and Eye Tracking study. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 17, Pages 52-62.

Scalf, P. E., & Beck, D. M. (2010). Competition in visual cortex impedes attention to multiple items. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 161-169.

Scalf, P. E., Basak, C., & Beck, D. M. (2011). Attention does more than modulate suppressive interactions: attending to multiple items. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 293-304.

Scangos, K. W., & Stuphorn, V. (2010). Medial frontal cortex motivates but does not control movement initiation in the countermanding task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1968-1982.

Schad, D. J., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2010). Eye movements during reading of randomly shuffled text. Vision Research, 50, 2600-2616.

Schad, D. J., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2012). Your mind wanders weakly, your mind wanders deeply: Objective measures reveal mindless reading at different levels. Cognition, 125, 179-194.

Scharff, A., Palmer, J., & Moore, C. M. (2013). Divided attention limits perception of 3-D object shapes. Journal of Vision, 13(2):18, 1-24, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/2/18, doi:10.1167/13.2.18.

Schattka, K. I., Radach, R., & Huber, W. (2010). Eye movement correlates of acquired central dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2959-2973.

Scheel, M., Abegg, M., Lanyon, L. J., Mattman, A., & Barton, J. J. (2010). Eye movement and diffusion tensor imaging analysis of treatment effects in a Niemann Pick Type C Patient. Molecular Genetics and Metabolism, 99, 291-295.

Scheepers, C., Keller, F., & Lapata, M. (2008). Evidence for serial coercion: A time course analysis using the visual-world paradigm. Cognitive Psychology, 56, 1-29.

Scheepers, C., Mohr, S., Fischer, M.H., & Roberts, A.M. (2013). Listening to limericks: A pupillometry investigation of perceivers' expectancy. PLoS ONE 8(9): e74986. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074986.

Scheller, E., Büchel, C., & Gamer, M. (2012). Diagnostic features of emotional expressions are processed preferentially. PLoS ONE 7(7): e41792. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041792.

Scherlen, A.-C., Bernard, J.-P., Calabrese, A., & Castet, E. (2008). Page mode reading with simulated scotomas: Oculo-motor patterns. Vision Research, 48, 1870-1878 .

Schiegg, A., Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. (2003). Attentional selection during preparation of prehension movements. Visual Cognition, 10, 409-431.

Schlag, J., Cai, R. H., Dorfman, A., Mohempour, A., & Schlag-Rey, M. (2000). Extrapolating movement without retinal motion. Nature, 403, 38-39.

Schleifer, P., & Landerl, K. (2011). Subitizing and counting in typical and atypical development. Developmental Science, 14, 280-291.

Schmalzl, L., Palermo, R., Green, M., Brunsdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Training of familiar face recognition and visual scan paths for faces in a child with congenital prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 704-729.

Schmechtig, A., Lees, J., Grayson, L., Craig, K. J., Dadhiwala, R., Dawson, G. R., Deakin, J. F., Dourish, C. T., Koychev, I., McMullen, K., Migo, E. M., Perry, C., Wilkinson, L., Morris, R., Williams, S. C., & Ettinger, U. (2013). Effects of risperidone, amisulpride and nicotine on eye movement control and their modulation by schizotypy. Psychopharmacology, 227, 331-345.

Schmechtig, A., Lees, J., Perkins, A., Altavilla, A., Craig, K.J., Dawson, G.R., Deakin, J.F., Dourish, C.T., Evans, L.H., Koychev, I., Weaver, K., Smallman, R., Walters, J., Wilkinson, L.S., Morris, R., Williams, S.C., & Ettinger, U. (2013). The effects of ketamine and risperidone on eye movement control in healthy volunteers. Translational Psychiatry (2013) 3, e334; doi:10.1038/tp.2013.109.

Schmidt, J., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2009). Search guidance is proportional to the categorical specificity of a target cue. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1904-1914.

Schmidt, J., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2011). Visual search guidance is best after a short delay. Vision Research, 51, 535-545.

Schmidt, L. J., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). The presence of threat affects saccade trajectories. Visual Cognition, 20, 284-299.

Schmidt, W. C. (2000). Endogenous attention and illusory line motion reexamined. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 980-996.

Schneider, D., Lam, R., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Cognitive load disrupts implicit theory-of-mind processing. Psychological Science, 23, 842-847.

Schneider, D., Bayliss, A. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Eye movements reveal sustained implicit processing of others' mental states. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 433-438.

Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129, 410-417.

Schneider, E., Maruyama, M., Dehaene, S., & Sigman, M. (2012). Eye gaze reveals a fast, parallel extraction of the syntax of arithmetic formulas. Cognition, 125, 475-490.

Schneider, M., Heine, A., Thaler, V., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., Jacobs, A., & Stern, E. (2008). A validation of eye movements as a measure of elementary school children's developing number sense. Cognitive Development, 23, 409-422.

Schneps, M.H., Thomson, J.M., Sonnert, G., Pomplun, M., Chen, C., & Heffner-Wong, A. (2013). Shorter lines facilitate reading in those who struggle. PLoS ONE 8(8): e71161. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0071161.

Schnier, F., & Lappe, M. (2011). Differences in inter-saccadic adaptation transfer between inward and outward adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 1399-1410.

Schnier, F., & Lappe, M. (2012). Mislocalization of stationary and ?ashed bars after saccadic inward and outward adaptation of reactive saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 3062-3070.

Schnier, F., Zimmermann, E., & Lappe, M. (2010). Adaptation and mislocalization fields for saccadic outward adaptation in humans. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(3):4, 1-18.

Schofield, C. A., Inhoff, A. W., & Coles, M. E. (2013). Time-course of attention biases in social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27, 661-669.

Schofield, C. A., Johnson, A. L., Inhoff, A. W., & Coles, M. E. (2012). Social anxiety and difficulty disengaging threat: Evidence from eye-tracking. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 300-311.

Schorer, J., Loffing, F., Hagemann, N., & Baker, J. (2012). Human handedness in interactive situations: Negative perceptual frequency effects can be reversed! Journal of Sports Sciences, 30, 507-513.

Schorer, J., Rienhoff, R., Fischer, L., & Baker, J. (2013). Foveal and peripheral fields of vision influences perceptual skill in anticipating opponents’ attacking position in volleyball. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38, 185-192.

Schotter, E. R. (2013). Synonyms provide semantic preview benefit in English. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 619-633.

Schotter, E. R., Berry, R. W., McKenzie, C. R. M., & Rayner, K. (2010). Gaze bias: Selective encoding and liking effects. Visual Cognition, 18, 1113-1132.

Schotter, E. R., Ferreira, V. S., & Rayner, K. (2013). Parallel object activation and attentional gating of information: Evidence From eye movements in the multiple object naming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition., 39, 365-374.

Schotter, E. R., Gerety, C., & Rayner, K. (2012). Heuristics and criterion setting during selective encoding in visual decision making: Evidence from eye movements. Visual Cognition, 20, 1110-1129.

Schrammel, F., Pannasch, S., Graupner, S.-T., Mojzisch, A., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2009). Virtual friend or threat? The effects of facial expression and gaze interaction on psychophysiological responses and emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 46, 922-931.

Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 651-664.

Schwarzbach, J. (2011). A simple framework (ASF) for behavioral and neuroimaging experiments based on the psychophysics toolbox for MATLAB. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 1194-1201.

Schütz, A. C. (2011). Motion transparency: Depth ordering and smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision, 11(14):21, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/21, doi:10.1167/11.14.21.

Schütz, A. C. (2012). There's more behind it: Perceived depth order biases perceived numerosity/density. Journal of Vision, 12(12):9, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/12/9, doi:10.1167/12.12.9.

Schütz, A. C., & Morrone, M. C. (2010). Compression of time during smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Research, 50, 2702-2713.

Schütz, A. C., & Souto, D. (2011). Adaptation of catch-up saccades during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 209, 537-549.

Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2007). Contrast sensitivity during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Research, 47, 2767-2777.

Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2009). Chromatic contrast sensitivity during optokinetic nystagmus, visually-enhanced vestibulo-ocular reflex and smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 2317-2327.

Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2009). Improved visual sensitivity during smooth pursuit eye movements: Temporal and spatial characteristics. Visual Neuroscience, 26, 329-340.

Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2009). Object recognition during foveating eye movements. Vision Research, 49, 2241-2253.

Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Improved visual sensitivity during smooth pursuit eye movements. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1211-1216.

Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., Movshon, J. A., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Does the noise matter? Effects of different kinematogram types on smooth pursuit eye movements and perception. Journal of Vision, 10(13):26, 1-22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/13/26, doi:10.1167/10.13.26.

Schütz, A. C., Delipetkos, E., Braun, D. I., Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2007). Temporal contrast sensitivity during smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision, 7(13):3, 1-15, http://journalofvision.org/7/13/3/, doi:10.1167/7.13.3.

Schütz, A. C., Trommershäuser, J., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Dynamic integration of information about salience and value for saccadic eye movements. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 7547-7552.

Schütz, A., Lossin, F., & Kerzel, D. (2013).Temporal stimulus properties that attract gaze to the periphery and repel gaze from fixation. Journal of Vision, 13(5):6, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/5/6, doi:10.1167/13.5.6.

Schütz, I., Henriques, D. Y. P., & Fiehler, K. (2013). Gaze-centered spatial updating in delayed reaching even in the presence of landmarks. Vision Research, 87, 46-52.

Sears, C. R., Campbell, C. R., & Lupker, S. J. (2006). Is there a neighborhood frequency effect in English? Evidence from reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 1040-1062.

Sears, C. R., Newman, K. R., Ference, J. D., & Thomas, C. L. (2011). Attention to emotional images in previously depressed individuals: An Eye-Tracking Study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 517-528.

Sears, C. R., Thomas, C. L., LeHuquet, J. M., & Johnson, J. C. S. (2010). Attentional biases in dysphoria: An eye-tracking study of the allocation and disengagement of attention. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 1349-1368.

Seeber, K. G., & Kerzel, D. (2012). Cognitive load in simultaneous interpreting: model meets data. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16, 228-242.

Seiler, M. J., Madhavan, P., & Liechty, M. (2012). Toward an understanding of real estate home buyer internet search behavior: An application of ocular tracking technology. Journal of Real Estate Research, 34, 211-242.

Seiler, M. J., Madhavan, P., & Liechty, m. (2012). Ocular tracking and the behavioral effects of negative externalities on perceived property values. Journal of Housing Research, 21, 123-137.

Selen, L. J. P. , & Medendorp, W. P. (2011). Saccadic updating of object orientation for grasping movements. Vision Research, 51, 898-907.

Serences, J. T., Schwarzbach, J., Courtney, S., Golay, X., & Yantis, S. (2004). Control of object-based attention in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1346-1357.

Seya, Y., & Watanabe, K. (2012). The minimal time required to process visual information in visual search tasks measured by using gaze-contingent visual masking. Perception, 41, 819-830.

Seya, Y., Nakayasu, H., & Patterson, P. (2008). Visual search of trained and untrained drivers in a driving simulator. Japanese Psychological Research, 50, 242-252.

Shadmehr, R., Orban de Xivry, J. J., Xu-Wilson, M., & Shih, T.-Y. (2010). Temporal discounting of reward and the cost of time in motor control. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 10507-10516.

Shake, M. C., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2011). Age differences in resolving anaphoric expressions during reading. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 18, 678-707.

Shakespeare, T.J., Yong, K.X.X., Frost, C., Kim, L.G., Warrington, E.K., & Crutch, S.J. (2013). Scene perception in posterior cortical atrophy: categorization, description and fixation patterns. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7:621. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00621.

Shalom, D. E., Dagnino, B., & Sigman, M. (2011). Looking at Breakout: Urgency and predictability direct eye events. Vision Research, 51, 1262-1272.

Shalom, D. E., & Sigman, M. (2013). Freedom and rules in human sequential performance: A refractory period in eye-hand coordination.Journal of Vision, 13(3):4, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/4, doi:10.1167/13.3.4.

Shalom, D.E., de Sousa Serro, M.G., Giaconia, M., Martinez, L.M., Rieznik, A., Sigman, M. (2013). Choosing in freedom or forced to choose? Introspective blindness to psychological forcing in stage-magic. PLoS ONE 8(3): e58254. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058254.

Shankar, S., Massoglia, D. P., Zhu, D., Costello, M. G., Stanford, T. R., & Salinas, E. (2011). Tracking the temporal evolution of a perceptual judgment using a compelled-response task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8406-8421.

Sharika, K.M., Neggers, S.F., Gutteling, T.P., Van der Stigchel, S., Dijkerman, H.C., & Murthy, A. (2013). Proactive control of sequential saccades in the human supplementary eye field. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, E1311-E1320.

Sharp, M. E., Viswanathan, J., Lanyon, L. J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2012). Sensitivity and bias in decision-making under risk: Evaluating the perception of reward, Its Probability and Value. PLoS ONE 7(4): e33460. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033460.

Shatzman, K. B., & McQueen, J. M. (2006). Prosodic knowledge affects the recognition of newly acquired words. Psychological Science, 17, 372-377.

Shatzman, K. B., & McQueen, J. M. (2006). Segment duration as a cue to word boundaries in spoken-word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1-16.

Shatzman, K. B., & McQueen, J. M. (2006). The modulation of lexical competition by segment duration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 966-971.

Shaw, T. A., & Porter, M. A. (2013). Emotion recognition and visual-scan paths in Fragile X Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1119-1139.

Sheikh, N. A., & Titone, D. A. (2013). Sensorimotor and linguistic information attenuate emotional word processing benefits: An eye-movement study. Emotion, 13, 1107-1121.

Sheldon, C. A., Abegg, M., Sekunova, A., & Barton, J. J. S. (2012). The word-length effect in acquired alexia, and real and virtual hemianopia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 841-851.

Shen, D., Liversedge, S. P., Tian, J., Zang, C., Cui, L., Bai, X., Yan, G., & Rayner, K. (2012). Eye movements of second language learners when reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 192-202.

Shen, J., Deutsch, D., & Rayner, K. (2013). On-line perception of Mandarin Tones 2 and 3: evidence from eye movements. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133(5):3016-3029.

Shen, J., Reingold, E. M., & Pomplun, M. (2000). Distractor ratio influences patterns of eye movements during visual search. Perception, 29, 241-250.

Shen, J., Reingold, E. M., & Pomplun, M. (2003). Guidance of eye movements during conjunctive visual search: The distractor-ratio effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 76-96.

Shen, K., & Paré, M. (2006). Guidance of eye movements during visual conjunction search: Local and global contextual effects on target discriminability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 2845-2855.

Shen, Z., Xu, W.-F., & Li, C.-Y. (2007). Cue-invariant detection of centre/surround discontinuity by V1 neurons in awake macaque monkey. Journal of Physiology, 583, 581-592.

Shepherd, S. V., Klein, J. T., Deaner, R. O., & Platt, M .L. (2009). Mirroring of attention by neurons in macaque parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 9489-9494.

Sheridan, H., & Reingold, E. M. (2012). Perceptual specificity effects in rereading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 255-269.

Sheridan, H., & Reingold, E. M. (2012). Perceptually specific and perceptually non-specific influences on rereading benefits for spatially transformed text: Evidence from eye movements. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1739-1747.

Sheridan, H., & Reingold, E. M. (2012). The time course of contextual influences during lexical ambiguity resolution: Evidence from distributional analyses of fixation durations. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1122-1131.

Sheridan, H., & Reingold, E. M. (2012). The time course of predictability effects in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Visual Cognition, 20, 733-745.

Sheridan, H., & Reingold, E. M. (2013). The mechanisms and boundary conditions of the Einstellung Effect in chess: Evidence from eye movements. PLoS ONE 8(10): e75796. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075796.

Sheridan, H., Rayner, K., & Reingold, E. M. (2013). Unsegmented text delays word identification: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Visual Cognition, 21, 38-60.

Sheridan, H., Reingold, E. M. (2013). A further examination of the lexical-processing stages hypothesized by the E-Z Reader model. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 407-414.

Sheridan, H., Reingold, E. M., & Daneman, M. (2009). Using puns to study contextual influences on lexical ambiguity resolution: Evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 875-881.

Shery, T., Proudlock, F. A., Sarvananthan, N., McLean, R. J., & Gottlob, I. (2006). The effects of gabapentin and memantine in acquired and congenital nystagmus: A retrospective study. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 90, 839-843.

Sheth, B. R., & Shimojo, S. (2004). Sound-aided recovery from and persistence against visual filling-in. Vision Research, 44, 1907-1917.

Sheth, V., Gottlob, I., Mohammad, S., McLean, R. J., Maconachie, G. D. E., Kumar, A., Degg, C., & Proudlock, F. A. (2013). Diagnostic potential of iris cross-sectional imaging in albinism using optical coherence tomography. Ophthalmology, 120, 2082-2090.

Shi, X.-F., F., Xu, L.-M., Li, Y., Wang, T., Zhao, K.-X., & Sabel, B. A. (2012). Fixational saccadic eye movements are altered in anisometropic amblyopia. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 30, 445-462.

Shi, Z., & Nijhawan, R. (2012). Motion extrapolation in the central fovea. PLoS ONE 7(3): e33651. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033651.

Shields, K., Engelhardt, P. E., & Ietswaart, M. (2012). Processing emotion information from both the face and body: An eye-movement study. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 699-709.

Shigihara, Y., & Zeki, S. (2013). Parallelism in the brain's visual form system. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 3712-3720.

Shih, S.-I., Meadmore, K. L., & Liversedge, S. P. (2012). Aging, eye movements, and object-location memory. PLoS ONE 7(3): e33485. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033485.

Shih, S.-I., Meadmore, K. L., & Liversedge, S. P. (2012). Using eye movement measures to investigate effects of age on memory for objects in a scene. Memory, 20, 629-637.

Shikauchi, M., Ishii, S., & Shibata, T. (2008). Prediction of aperiodic target sequences by saccades. Behavioural Brain Research, 189, 325-331.

Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., & Scheier, C. (2003). Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1317-1322.

Shimono, M., & Niki, K. (2013). Global mapping of the whole-brain network underlining binocular rivalry. Brain Connectivity, 3, 212-221.

Shimono, M., Mano, H., & Niki, K. (2012). The brain structural hub of interhemispheric information integration for visual motion perception. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 337 - 344.

Shimozaki, S. S., Schoonveld, W. A., & Eckstein, M. P. (2012). A unified Bayesian observer analysis for set size and cueing effects on perceptual decisions and saccades. Journal of Vision, 12(6):27, 1-26, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/6/27, doi:10.1167/12.6.27.

Shu, H., Zhou, W., Yan, M., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Font size modulates saccade-target selection in Chinese reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 482-490.

Siebold, A., van Zoest, W., Meeter, M., & Donk, M. (2013). In defense of the salience map: Salience rather than visibility determines selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 1516-1524.

Siebold, A., van Zoest, W., & Donk, M. (2011). Oculomotor evidence for top-down control following the initial saccade. PLoS ONE 6(9): e23552. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023552.

Sigman, M., Sackur, J., Del Cul, A., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Illusory displacement due to object substitution near the consciousness threshold. Journal of Vision, 8(1):13, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/8/1/13/, doi:10.1167/8.1.13.

Silver, M. A., Shenhav, A., & D'Esposito, M. (2008). Cholinergic enhancement reduces spatial spread of visual responses in human early visual cortex. Neuron, 60, 904-914.

Silvetti, M., Seurinck, R., van Bochove, M.E., & Verguts, T. (2013). The influence of the noradrenergic system on optimal control of neural plasticity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7:160. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00160.

Simac-Lejeune, A., Marat, S., Pellerin, D., Lambert, P., Rombaut, M., & Guyader, N. (2009). Relevance of interest points for eye position prediction on videos. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5815, 325-334.

Simion, C., & Shimojo, S. (2006). Early interactions between orienting, visual sampling and decision making in facial preference. Vision Research, 46, 3331-3335.

Simion, C., & Shimojo, S. (2007). Interrupting the cascade: Orienting contributes to decision making even in the absence of visual stimulation. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 591-595.

Simoncini, C., Perrinet, L. U., Montagnini, A., Mamassian, P. & Masson, G. S. (2012). More is not always better: adaptive gain control explains dissociation between perception and action. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 1596-1603.

Simpson, S. A., Abegg, M., & Barton, J. J. S. (2011). Rapid adaptation of visual search in simulated hemianopia. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1593-1601.

Sims, C. R., Jacobs, R. A., & Knill, D. C. (2012). An ideal observer analysis of visual working memory. Psychological Review, 119, 807-830.

Sims, C. R., Knill, D. C., & Jacobs, R. A. (2011). Adaptive allocation of vision under competing task demands. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 928-943.

Singer, J. M., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2010). Temporal cortex neurons encode articulated actions as slow sequences of integrated poses. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3133-3145.

Sinn, P., & Engbert, R. (2011). Saccadic facilitation by modulation of microsaccades in natural backgrounds. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1029-1033.

Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & Schmitt, N. (2011). Adding more fuel to the fire: An eye-tracking study of idiom processing by native and non-native speakers. Second Language Research, 27, 251-272.

Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2011). Seeing a phrase "time and again" matters: The role of phrasal frequency in the processing of multiword sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 776-784.

Slattery, T. J. (2009). Word misperception, the neighbor frequency effect, and the role of sentence context: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 1969-1975.

Slattery, T. J., & Rayner, K. (2010). The influence of text legibility on eye movements during reading. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1129-1148.

Slattery, T. J., & Rayner, K. (2013). Effects of intraword and interword spacing on eye movements during reading: Exploring the optimal use of space in a line of text. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1275-1292.

Slattery, T. J., Angele, B., & Rayner, K. (2011). Eye movements and display change detection during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1924-1938.

Slattery, T. J., Schotter, E. R., Berry, R. W., & Rayner, K. (2011). Parafoveal and foveal processing of abbreviations during eye fixations in reading: Making a case for case. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1022-1031.

Slattery, T. J., Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2012). Saccade launch site as a predictor of fixation durations in reading: Comments on Hand, Miellet, O'Donnell, and Sereno (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 251-261.

Slattery, T. J., Sturt, P., Christianson, K., Yoshida, M., & Ferreira, F. (2013). Lingering misinterpretations of garden path sentences arise from competing syntactic representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 104-120.

Slotnick, S. D., & Yantis, S. (2005). Common neural substrates for the control and effects of visual attention and perceptual bistability. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 97-108.

Slotnick, S. D., Hopfinger, J. B., Klein, S. A., & Sutter E. E. (2002). Darkness beyond the light: Attentional inhibition surrounding the classic spotlight. NeuroReport, 13, 773-778.

Slotnick, S. D., Schwarzbach,, J., & Yantis, S. (2003). Attentional inhibition of visual processing in human striate and extrastriate cortex. NeuroImage, 19, 1602-1611.

Smeets, E., Jansen, A., & Roefs, A. (2011). Bias for the (un)attractive self: On the role of attention in causing body (dis)satisfaction. Health Psychology, 30, 360-367.

Smeets, J. B. J., & Hooge, I. T. C. (2003). Nature of variability in saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 12-20.

Smerecnik, C. M. R., Mesters, I., Kessels, L. T. E., Ruiter, R. A. C., De Vries, N. K., & De Vries, H. (2010). Understanding the positive effects of graphical risk information on comprehension: Measuring attention directed to written, tabular, and graphical risk information. Risk Analysis, 30, 1387-1398.

Smilek, D., Birmingham, E., Cameron, D., Bischof, W., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Cognitive ethology and exploring attention in real-world scenes. Brain Research, 1080, 101-119.

Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S. A., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Out of mind, out of sight: Eye blinking as indicator and embodiment of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 21, 786-789.

Smilek, D., Malcolmson, K. A., Carriere, J. S. A., Eller, M., Kwan, D., & Reynolds, M. (2007). When "3" is a Jerk and "E" is a King: Personifying Inanimate Objects in Synesthesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 981-992.

Smilek, D., Solman, G. J. F., Murawski, P., & Carriere, J. S. A. (2009). The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 57-61.

Smith, M. A. B., Youmans, R. J., Bellows, B. G., & Peterson, M. S. (2013). Shifting the focus: An objective look at design fixation. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8012, 144-151.

Smith, N. D., Crabb, D. P., & Garway-Heath, D. F. (2011). An exploratory study of visual search performance in glaucoma. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics. 31, 225-232.

Smith, N. D., Glen, F. C., & Crabb, D. P. (2012). Eye movements during visual search in patients with glaucoma. BMC Ophthalmology, 12, 45.

Smith, T. J. (2012). The attentional theory of cinematic continuity, projections. The Journal for Movies and the Mind. 6(1), 1-27.

Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2008). Edit Blindness: The relationship between attention and global change blindness in dynamic scenes. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(2):6, 1-17.

Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Facilitation of return during scene viewing. Visual Cognition, 17, 1083-1108.

Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). Does oculomotor inhibition of return influence fixation probability during scene search? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 2384-2398.

Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). Looking back at Waldo: Oculomotor inhibition of return does not prevent return fixations. Journal of Vision, 11(1):3, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/1/3, doi:10.1167/11.1.3.

Smith, T. J., & Mital, P. K. (2013). Attentional synchrony and the influence of viewing task on gaze behavior in static and dynamic scenes. Journal of Vision, 13(8):16, 1-24, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/16, doi:10.1167/13.8.16.

Smith, T. J., Lamont, P., & Henderson, J. M. (2012). The penny drops: Change blindness at fixation. Perception, 41, 489-492.

Smith, T. J., Lamont, P., & Henderson, J. M. (2013). Change blindness in a dynamic scene due to endogenous override of exogenous attentional cues. Perception, 42, 884-886.

So, N.-Y., & Stuphorn, V. (2010). Supplementary eye field encodes option and action value for saccades with variable reward. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 2634-2653.

So, N.-Y., & Stuphorn, V. (2012). Supplementary eye field encodes reward prediction error. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 2950-2963.

Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (2008). Extrapolation of visual motion for manual interception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99, 2956-2967.

Soechting, J. F., Engel, K. C., & Flanders, M. (2001). The Duncker illusion and eye-hand coordination. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85, 843-854.

Soechting, J. F., Juveli, J. Z., & Rao, H. M. (2009). Models for the extrapolation of target motion for manual interception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 1491 - 1502.

Soechting, J. F., Mrotek, L. A., & Flanders, M. (2005). Smooth pursuit tracking of an abrupt change in target direction: vector superposition of discrete responses. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 245-258.

Soechting, J. F., Rao, H. M., & Juveli, J. Z. (2010). Incorporating prediction in models for two-dimensional smooth pursuit. PLoS ONE 5(9): e12574. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012574.

Sogo, H. (2013). GazeParser: an open-source and multiplatform library for low-cost eye tracking and analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 684-695.

Sogo, H., & Takeda, Y. (2006). Effect of previously fixated locations on saccade trajectory during free visual search. Vision Research, 46, 3831-3844.

Sogo, H., & Takeda, Y. (2007). Saccade trajectory under simultaneous inhibition for two locations. Vision Research, 47, 1537-1549.

Sogo, H., & Takeda, Y. (2009). Effect of spatial inhibition on saccade trajectory depends on location-based mechanisms. Japanese Psychological Research, 51, 35-46.

Soliman, A., O'Driscoll1, G. A., Pruessner, J., Holahan, A.-L. V., Boileau, I., Gagnon, D., & Dagher, A. (2008). Stress-induced dopamine release in humans at risk of psychosis: a [11C]Raclopride PET study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 2033-2041.

Solman, G. J. F., & Smilek, D. (2010). Item-specific location memory in visual search. Vision Research, 50, 2430-2438.

Solman, G. J. F., & Smilek, D. (2012). Memory benefits during visual search depend on difficulty. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 689-702.

Solman, G. J. F., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2011). Memory load affects visual search processes without influencing search efficiency. Vision Research, 51, 1185-1191.

Solman, G. J. F., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2012). Found and missed: Failing to recognize a search target despite moving it. Cognition, 123, 100-118.

Solé Puig, M., Pérez Zapata, L., Aznar-Casanova, J.A., & Supèr, H. (2013). A role of eye vergence in covert attention. PLoS ONE 8(1): e52955. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052955.

Sommerhalder, G., Oueghlani, E., Bagnoud, B., Leonards, U., Safran, A. B., & Pelizzone, P. (2003). Simulation of artificial vision: I. Eccentric reading of isolated words, and perceptual learning. Vision Research, 43, 269-283.

Sommerhalder, J., Rappaz, B., de Haller, R., Fornos, A. P., Safran, A. B., & Pelizzone, M. (2004). Simulation of artificial vision: II. Eccentric reading of full-page text and the learning of this task. Vision Research, 44, 1693-1706.

Song, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S., Lutti, A., Li, B., Kanai, R., & Rees, G. (2013). Effective connectivity within human primary visual cortex predicts interindividual diversity in illusory perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 18781-18791.

Song, G., Pellerin, D., & Granjon, L. (2013). Different types of sounds influence gaze differently in videos. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 6(4):1, 1-13.

Song, J.-H., & Bédard, P. (2013). Allocation of attention for dissociated visual and motor goals. Experimental Brain Research, 226, 209-219.

Song, J.-H., & McPeek, R. M. (2009). Eye-hand coordination during target selection in a pop-out visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 2681-2692.

Song, J.-H., & McPeek, R. M. (2010). Roles of narrow- and broad-spiking dorsal premotor area neurons in reach target selection and movement production. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 2124-2138.

Song, J.-H., & Nakayama, K. (2007). Fixation offset facilitates saccades and manual reaching for single but not multiple target displays. Experimental Brain Research, 177, 223-232.

Song, J.-H., Rafal, R. D., & McPeek, R. M. (2011). Deficits in reach target selection during inactivation of the midbrain superior colliculus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, E1433-E1440.

Song, M., Tao, D., Chen, C., Bu, J., & Yang, Y. (2013). Color-to-gray based on chance of happening preservation. Neurocomputing, 119, 222-231.

Sorrento, G. U., & Henriques, D. Y. P. (2008). Reference frame conversions for repeated arm movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99, 2968-2984.

Souman, J. L., & Freeman, T. C. A. (2008). Motion perception during sinusoidal smooth pursuit eye movements: Signal latencies and non-linearities. Journal of Vision, 8(14):10, 1-14, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/10/, doi:10.1167/8.14.10.

Souman, J. L., Hooge, I. Th. C., & Wertheim, A. H. (2005). Perceived motion direction during smooth pursuit eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 164, 376-386.

Souman, J. L., Hooge, I. Th. C., & Wertheim, A. H. (2005). Vertical object motion during horizontal ocular pursuit: Compensation for eye movements increases with presentation duration. Vision Research, 45, 845-853.

Souman, J. L., Hooge, I. Th. C., & Wertheim, A. H. (2006). Frame of reference transformations in motion perception during smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Computational Neuroscience, 20, 61-76.

Souman, J. L., Hooge, I. Th. C., & Wertheim, A. H. (2006). Localization and motion perception during smooth pursuit eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 171, 448-458.

Souto, D., & Kerzel, D. (2008). Dynamics of attention during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision, 8(14):3, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/3/, doi:10.1167/8.14.3.

Souto, D., & Kerzel, D. (2009). Evidence for an attentional component in saccadic inhibition of return. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 531-540.

Souto, D., & Kerzel, D. (2009). Involuntary cueing effects during smooth pursuit: facilitation and inhibition of return in oculocentric coordinates. Experimental Brain Research, 192, 25-31.

Souto, D., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Like a rolling stone: Naturalistic visual kinematics facilitate tracking eye movements. Journal of Vision, 13(2):9, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/13/2/9, doi:10.1167/13.2.9.

Spakov, O., & Majaranta, P. (2009). Scrollable keyboards for casual eye typing. PsychNology Journal, 7, 159-173.

Sperandio, I., Kaderali, S., Chouinard, P. A., Frey, J., & Goodale, M. A. (2013). Perceived size change induced by nonvisual signals in darkness: The relative contribution of vergence and proprioception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 16915-16923.

Spering, M., & Carrasco, M. (2012). Similar effects of feature-based Attention on motion perception and pursuit eye movements at different levels of awareness. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 7594-7601.

Spering, M., Dias, E. C., Sanchez, J. L., Schütz, A. C., & Javitt, D. C. (2013). Efference copy failure during smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 11779-11787.

Spering, M., Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Kerzel, D. (2006). Distractor interference during smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1136-1154.

Spering, M., Kerzel, D., Braun, D. I., Hawken,. M. J., & Gegenfurtner, K. R., (2005). Effects of contrast on smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision. 5, 455-465. http://journalofvision.org/5/5/6/

Spering, M., Montagnini, A., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Competition between color and luminance for target selection in smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements. Journal of Vision, 8(15):16, 1-19, http://journalofvision.org/8/15/16/, doi:10.1167/8.15.16.

Spering, M., Pomplun, M., & Carrasco, M. (2011). Tracking without perceiving: A dissociation between eye movements and motion perception. Psychological Science, 22, 216-225.

Spering, M., Schütz, A. C., Braun, D. I., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Keep your eyes on the ball: Smooth pursuit eye movements enhance prediction of visual motion. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 1756-1767.

Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S. E., & Piven, J. (2007). Abnormal use of facial information in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 929-939.

Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S. E., & Piven, J. (2007). Analysis of face gaze in autism using "Bubbles". Neuropsychologia, 45, 144-151.

Spezio, M. L., Huang, P.-Y. S., Castelli, F., & Adolphs, R. (2007). Amygdala damage impairs eye contact during conversations with real people. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3994-3997.

Spitzyna, G. A., Wise, R. J.S., McDonald, S. A., Plant, G. T., Kidd, D., Crewes, H., & Leff, A. P. (2007). Optokinetic therapy improves text reading in patients with hemianopic alexia: A controlled trial. Neurology, 68, 1922-1930.

Sprenger, A., Friedrich, M., Nagel, M., Schmidt, C.S., Moritz, S., & Lencer, R. (2013). Advanced analysis of free visual exploration patterns in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:737. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00737.

Sprenger, A., Lappe-Osthege, M., Talamo, S., Gais, S., Kimmig, H., & Helmchen, C. (2010). Eye movements during REM sleep and imagination of visual scenes. NeuroReport, 21, 45-49.

Sprenger, A., Trillenberg, P., Nagel, M., Sweeney, J. A., & Lencer, R. (2013). Enhanced top-down control during pursuit eye tracking in schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, 223-231.

Sprenger, A., Trillenberg, P., Pohlmann, J., Herold, K., Lencer, R., & Helmchen, C. (2011). The role of prediction and anticipation on age-related effects on smooth pursuit eye movements. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1233, 168-176.

Sprenger, A., Zils, E., Rambold, H., Sander, T., & Helmchen, C. (2005). Effect of 3,4-Diaminopyridine on the postural control in patients with downbeat nystagmus. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039, 395-403.

Srimal, R., & Curtis, C. E. (2010). Secondary adaptation of memory-guided saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 206, 35-46.

Srivastava, S., Orban, G. A., De Mazière, P. A., & Janssen, P. (2009). A distinct representation of three-dimensional shape in macaque anterior intraparietal area: Fast, metric, and coarse. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 10613-10626.

Stainer, M.J., Scott-Brown, K.C., & Tatler, B.W. (2013). Behavioral biases when viewing multiplexed scenes: scene structure and frames of reference for inspection. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00624.

Stanford, T. R., Shankar, S., Massoglia, D. P., Costello, M. G., & Salinas, E. (2010). Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 379-385.

Starzynski, C., & Engbert, R. (2009). Noise-enhanced target discrimination under the influence of fixational eye movements and external noise. Chaos, 19, 015112: 1-7.

Staub, A. (2007). The parser doesn't ignore intransitivity, after all. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 550-569.

Staub, A. (2010). Eye movements and processing difficulty in object relative clauses. Cognition, 116, 71-86.

Staub, A. (2011). The effect of lexical predictability on distributions of eye fixation durations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 371-376.

Staub, A. (2011). Word recognition and syntactic attachment in reading: Evidence for a staged architecture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 407-433.

Staub, A., & Benatar, A. (2013). Individual differences in fixation duration distributions in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1304-1311.

Staub, A., Abbott, M., & Bogartz, R. S. (2012). Linguistically guided anticipatory eye movements in scene viewing. Visual Cognition, 20, 922-946.

Staub, A., Grant, M., Clifton, C., & Rayner, K. (2009). Phonological typicality does not influence fixation durations in normal reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 35, 806-814.

Staudte, M., & Crocker, M. W. (2011). Investigating joint attention mechanisms through spoken human-robot interaction. Cognition, 120, 268-291.

Steenbergen, B., Verrel, J., & Gordon, A. M. (2007). Motor planning in congenital hemiplegia. Disability & Rehabilitation, 29, 13-23.

Steenken, R., Colonius, H., Diederich, A., & Rach, S. (2008). Visual-auditory interaction in saccadic reaction time: Effects of auditory masker level. Brain Research, 1220, 150-156.

Steenken, R., Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2008). Time course of auditory masker effects: Tapping the locus of audiovisual integration? Neuroscience Letters, 435, 78-83.

Stein, T., Vallines, I., & Schneider, W. X. (2008). Primary visual cortex reflects behavioral performance in the attentional blink. NeuroReport, 19, 1277-1281.

Steiner, G. Z., & Barry, R. J. (2011). Pupillary responses and event-related potentials as indices of the orienting reflex. Psychophysiology, 48, 1648-1655.

Stephan, D.N., Koch, I., Hendler, J., & Huestegge, L. (2013). Task switching, modality compatibility, and the supra-modal function of eye movements. Experimental Psychology, 60, 90-99.

Stephen, D. G., & Mirman, D. (2010). Interactions dominate the dynamics of visual cognition. Cognition, 115, 154-165.

Stevens, C., Winskel, H., Howell, C., Vidal, L.-M., Latimer, C., Milne-Home, J. (2010). Perceiving dance: Schematic expectations guide experts' scanning of a contemporary dance film. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, 14, 19-25.

Stewart, A. J., Haigh, M., Ferguson, H. J. (2013). Sensitivity to speaker control in the online comprehension of conditional tips and promises: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1022-1036.

Stieff, M., Hegarty, M., & Deslongchamps, G. (2011). Identifying representational competence with multi-representational displays. Cognition and Instruction, 29, 123-145.

Stine-Morrow, E. A., Shake, M. C., Miles, J. R., Lee, K., Gao, X., & McConkie, G. (2010). Pay now or pay later: Aging and the role of boundary salience in self-regulation of conceptual integration in sentence processing.. Psychology & Aging, 25, 168-176.

Stites, M. C., Federmeier, K., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2013). Cross-age comparisons reveal multiple strategies for lexical ambiguity resolution during natural teading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1823-1841.

Stites, M. C., Luke, S. G., & Christianson, K. (2013). The psychologist said quickly, "Dialogue descriptions modulate reading speed!" Memory & Cognition, 41, 137-151.

Stojanoski, B., & Niemeier, M. (2007). Feature-based attention modulates the perception of object contours. Journal of Vision, 7(14):18, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/7/14/18/, doi:10.1167/7.14.18.

Stonkute, S., Braun, J., & Pastukhov, A. (2012). The role of attention in ambiguous reversals of structure-from-motion. PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e37734.

Stork, S., & Müsseler, J. (2004). Perceived localizations and eye movements with action-generated and computer-generated vanishing points of moving stimuli. Visual Cognition, 11, 299-314.

Stork, S., & Schubö, A. (2010). Human cognition in manual assembly: Theories and applications. Advanced Engineering Informatics, 24, 320-328.

Stork, S., Hild, I., Wiesbeck, M., Zaeh, M. F., & Schubö, A. (2009). Finding relevant items: Attentional guidance improves visual selection processes. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5889, 69-80.

Stork, S., Neggers, S. F. W., & Muesseler, J. (2002). Intentionally-evoked modulations of smooth pursuit eye movements. Human Movement Science, 21, 335-348.

Stoyanova, R. S., Pratt, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Inhibition of return to social signals of fear. Emotion, 7, 49-56.

Stritzke, M., & Trommershäuser, J. (2007). Eye movements during rapid pointing under risk. Vision Research, 47, 2000-2009.

Stritzke, M., Trommershäuser, J., & Gegenfurtner, K. (2009). Effects of salience and reward information during saccadic decisions under risk. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 26, B1-B13.

Stroud, M. J., Menneer, T., Cave, K. R., Donnelly, N., & Rayner, K. (2011). Search for multiple targets of different colours: Misguided eye movements reveal a reduction of colour selectivity. Applied cognitive Psychology, 25, 971-982.

Stroud, M. J., Menneer, T., Cave, K. R., & Donnelly, N. (2012). Using the dual-target cost to explore the nature of search target representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 113-122.

Sturt, P., Keller, F., & Dubey, A. (2010). Syntactic priming in comprehension: Parallelism effects with and without coordination. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 333-351.

Störmer, V. S., Li, S.-C., Heekeren, H. R., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Feature-based interference from unattended visual field during attentional tracking in younger and older adults. Journal of Vision, 11(2):1, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/2/1, doi:10.1167/11.2.1.

Su, Y., Rao, L.-L., Li, X., Wang, Y., & Li, S. (2012). From quality to quantity: The role of common features in consumer preference. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33, 1043-1058.

Su, Y., Rao, L.-L., Sun, H.-Y., Du, X.-L., Li, X., & Li, S. (2013). Is making a risky choice based on a weighting and adding process? An eye-tracking investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1765-1780.

Suh, M., Basu, S., Kolster, R., Sarkar, R., McCandliss, B., & Ghajar, J. (2006). Increased oculomotor deficits during target blanking as an indicator of mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroscience Letters, 410, 203-207.

Suh, M., Kolster, R., Sarkar, R., McCandliss, B., & Ghajar, J. (2006). Deficits in predictive smooth pursuit after mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroscience Letters, 401, 108-113.

Sullivan, B., Jovancevic, J., Hayhoe, M., & Sterns, G. (2005). Use of gaze in natural tasks in Stargardt's disease: A preferred retinal region. International Congress Series, 1282, 608-612.

Sullivan, B., Jovancevic-Misic, J., Hayhoe, M., & Sterns, G. (2008). Use of multiple preferred retinal loci in Stargardt's disease during natural tasks: a case study. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 28, 168-177.

Sullivan, J. L., Juhasz, B. J., Slattery, T. J., & Barth, H. C. (2011). Adults' number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 557-563.

Sullivan, S., Ruffman, T., & Hutton, S. B. (2007). Age differences in emotion recognition skills and the visual scanning of emotion faces. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, 53-60.

Sulpizio, S., & McQueen, J. M. (2012). Italians use abstract knowledge about lexical stress during spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 177-193.

Sun, P., Gardner, J. L., Costagli, M., Ueno, K., Waggoner, R. A., Tanaka, K., & Cheng, K.. (2013). Demonstration of tuning to stimulus orientation in the human visual cortex: a high-resolution FMRI study with a novel continuous and periodic stimulation paradigm. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1618-1629.

Sung, Y.-C., & Tang, D.-L. (2007). Unconscious processing embedded in conscious processing: Evidence from gaze time on Chinese sentence reading. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 339-348.

Susskind, J., Lee, D., Cusi, A., Feinman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 843-850.

Suzuki, M., Wolfe, J. M., Horowitz, T. S., & Noguchi, Y. (2013). Apparent color-orientation bindings in the periphery can be influenced by feature binding in central vision. Vision Research, 82, 58-65.

Svede, A., Hoormann,J., Jainta, S., & Jaschinski, W. (2011). Subjective fixation disparity affected by dynamic asymmetry, resting vergence, and nonius bias Investigative Ophthalmology, & Visual Science, 52, 4356-4361.

Swaminathan, S. K., & Freedman, D. J. (2012). Preferential encoding of visual categories in parietal cortex compared with prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 315-320.

Swaminathan, S. K., Masse, N. Y., & Freedman, D. J. (2013). A comparison of lateral and medial intraparietal areas during a visual categorization task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 13157-13170.

Szarkowska, A., Krejtz, I., Klyszejko, Z., & Wieczorek, A. (2011). Verbatim, standard, or edited?: Reading patterns of different captioning styles among deaf, hard of hearing, and hearing viewers. American Annals of the Deaf, 156, 363-378.

Szinte, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2011). Spatiotopic apparent motion reveals local variations in space constancy. Journal of Vision, 11(2):4, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/2/4, doi:10.1167/11.2.4.

Szinte, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2012). Apparent motion from outside the visual field, retinotopic cortices may register extra-retinal positions. PLoS ONE 7(10): e47386. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047386.

Szinte, M., Wexler, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2012). Temporal dynamics of remapping captured by peri-saccadic continuous motion. Journal of Vision, 12(7):12, 1-18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/7/12, doi:10.1167/12.7.12.

Back To Top

T

't Hart, B. M., Abresch, T. G. J., & Einhäuser, W. (2011). Faces in places: Humans and machines make similar face detection errors. PLoS ONE, 6(10): e25373. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025373.

't Hart, B. M., Schmidt, H. C. E. F., Klein-Harmeyer, I., & Einhäuser, W. (2013). Attention in natural scenes: contrast affects rapid visual processing and fixations alike. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130067. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0067.

't Hart, B. M., Schmidt, H.C.E.F., Roth, C., & Einhäuser, W. (2013). Fixations on objects in natural scenes: dissociating importance from salience. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:455. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00455.

Tadros, K., Dupuis-Roy, N., Fiset, D., Arguin, M., & Gosselin, F. (2013). Reading laterally: The cerebral hemispheric use of spatial frequencies in visual word recognition. Journal of Vision, 13(1):4, 1-12,http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/1/4, doi: 10.1167/13.1.4.

Tai, Z., Hertle, R. W., Bilonick, R. A., & Yang, D. (2011). A new algorithm for automated nystagmus acuity function analysis. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 95, 832-836.

Taibbi, G., Wang, Z. I., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2008). Infantile nystagmus syndrome: Broadening the high-foveation-quality field with contact lenses. Clinical Ophthalmology, 2, 585-589.

Takahashi, K., & Watanabe, K. (2008). Persisting effect of prior experience of change blindness. Perception, 37, 324-327.

Takahashi, K., Fukuda, H., Ikeda, H., Doi, H., Watanabe, K., Ueda, K., & Shinohara, K. (2011). Roles of the upper and lower bodies in direction discrimination of point-light walkers. Journal of Vision, 11(14):8, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/8, doi:10.1167/11.14.8.

Takemura, H., Ashida, H., Amano, K., Kitaoka, A., & Murakami, I. (2012). Neural correlates of induced motion perception in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 14344-14354.

Talsma, D., White, B.J., Mathôt, S., Munoz, D. P., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). A retinotopic attentional trace after saccadic eye movements: evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1563-1577.

Tan, H.-R. M., Leuthold, H., & Gross, J. (2013). Gearing up for action: Attentive tracking dynamically tunes sensory and motor oscillations in the alpha and beta band. NeuroImage, 82, 634-644.

Tang, S., Reilly, R. G., & Vorstius, C. (2012). EyeMap: a software system for visualizing and analyzing eye movement data in reading. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 420-438.

Tant, M. L., Cornelissen, F. W., Kooijman, A. C., & Brouwer, W. H. (2002). Hemianopic visual field defects elicit hemianopic scanning. Vision Research, 42, 1339-1348.

Tarasewich, P., Pomplun, M., Fillion, S., & Broberg, D. (2005). The enhanced restricted focus viewer. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 2005, 19, 35-54.

Tarita-Nistor, L., Brent, M. H., Steinbach, M. J., & Gonzalez, E. G. (2011). Fixation stability during binocular viewing in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 52, 1887-1893.

Tarita-Nistor, L., Brent, M. H., Steinbach, M. J., & González, E. G. (2012). Fixation patterns in maculopathy: From binocular to monocular viewing. Optometry & Vision Science, 89, 277-287.

Tark, K.-J., & Curtis, C. E. (2013). Deciding where to look based on visual, auditory, and semantic information. Brain Research, 1525, 26-38.

Tas, A. C., Dodd, M., D., & Hollingworth, A. (2012). The role of surface feature continuity in object-based inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 20, 29-47.

Tas, A. C., Moore, C. M., & Hollingworth, A. (2012). An object-mediated updating account of insensitivity to transsaccadic change. Journal of Vision, 12(11):18, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/11/18, doi:10.1167/12.11.18.

Tatler, B. W. (2007). The central fixation bias in scene viewing: Selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions. Journal of Vision, 7(14):4, 1-17, http://journalofvision.org/7/14/4/, doi:10.1167/7.14.4.

Tatler, B. W., & Hutton, S. B. (2007). Trial by trial effects in the antisaccade task. Experimental Brain Research, 179, 387-396.

Tatler, B. W., & Vincent, B. T. (2008). Systematic tendencies in scene viewing. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(2):5, 1-18.

Tatler, B. W., & Vincent, B. T. (2009). The prominence of behavioural biases in eye guidance. Visual Cognition, 17, 1029-1054.

Tatler, B. W., Baddeley, R. J., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2005). Visual correlates of fixation selection: Effects of scale and time. Vision Research, 45, 643-659.

Tatler, B. W., Baddeley, R. J., & Vincent, B. T. (2006). The long and the short of it: spatial statistics at fixation vary with saccade amplitude and task. Vision Research, 46, 1857-1862.

Tatler, B. W., Gilchrist, I. D., & Land, M. F. (2005). Visual memory for objects in natural scenes: From fixations to object files. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 931-960.

Tatler, B. W., Wade, N .J., & Kaulard, K. (2007). Examining art: dissociating pattern and perceptual influences on oculomotor behaviour. Spatial Vision, 21, 165-184.

Tatler, B. W., Wade, N. J., Kwan, H., Findlay, J. M., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2010). Yarbus, eye movements, and vision. i-Perception, 1, 7-27.

Tatler, B.W., Hirose, Y., Finnegan, S.K., Pievilainen, R., Kirtley, C., & Kennedy, A. (2013). Priorities for selection and representation in natural tasks. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130066. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0066.

Taubert, J., Marsh, P. J., & Shaw, T. (2010). When you turn the other cheek: A preference for novel viewpoints of familiar faces. Perception, 39, 429-432.

Tavassoli, A., & Ringach, D. L. (2009). Dynamics of smooth pursuit maintenance. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 110-118.

Tavassoli, A., & Ringach, D. L. (2010). When your eyes see more than you do. Current Biology, 20, R93-R94.

Taya, S., Windridge, D., & Osman, M. (2012). Looking to score: The dissociation of goal influence on eye movement and meta-attentional allocation in a complex dynamic natural scene. PLoS ONE 7(6): e39060. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039060.

Taya, S., Windridge, D., & Osman, M. (2013). Trained eyes: Experience promotes adaptive gaze control in dynamic and uncertain visual environments. PLoS ONE 8(8): e71371. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0071371.

Taylor, A. J. G., & Hutton, S. B. (2007). The effects of individual differences on cued antisaccade performance. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(1):5, 1-9.

Taylor, A. J. G., & Hutton, S. B. (2009). The effects of task instructions on pro and antisaccade performance. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 5-14.

Taylor, A. J. G., & Hutton, S. B. (2011). Error awareness and antisaccade performance. Experimental Brain Research, 213, 27-34.

Tchernikov, I., & Fallah, M. (2010). A color hierarchy for automatic target selection. PLoS ONE 5(2): e9338. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009338.

Teichert, T., Klingenhoefer, S., Wachtler, T., & Bremmer, F. (2008). Depth perception during saccades. Journal of Vision, 8(14):27, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/27/, doi:10.1167/8.14.27.

Telling, A. L., Meyer, A. S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2010). Distracted by relatives: Effects of frontal lobe damage on semantic distraction. Brain and Cognition, 73, 203-214.

Terao, M., & Murakami, I. (2011). Compensation for equiluminant color motion during smooth pursuit eye movement. Journal of Vision, 11(6):12, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/6/12, doi:10.1167/11.6.12.

Terao, M., Watanabe, J., Yagi, A., & Nishida, S. (2008). Reduction of stimulus visibility compresses apparent time intervals. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 541-542.

Terao, M., Watanabe, J., Yagi, A., & Nishida, S. (2010). Smooth pursuit eye movements improve temporal resolution for color perception. PLoS ONE 5(6): e11214. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011214.

Terao, Y., Fukuda, H., Shirota, Y., Yugeta, A., Yoshioka, M., Suzuki, M., Hanajima, R., Nomura, Y., Segawa, M., Tsuji, S., & Ugawa, Y. (2013). Deterioration of horizontal saccades in progressive supranuclear palsy. Clinical Neurophysiology, 124, 354-363.

Thakkar, K. N., Schall, J. D., Boucher, L., Logan, G. D., & Park, S. (2011). Response inhibition and response monitoring in a saccadic countermanding task in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 55-62.

Thaler, L., & Goodale, M. A. (2011). Reaction times for allocentric movements are 35 ms slower than reaction times for target-directed movements. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 313-328.

Thaler, L., Schütz, A. C., Goodale, M. A., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). What is the best fixation target? The effect of target shape on stability of fixational eye movements. Vision Research, 76, 31-42.

Thaler, L., Todd, J. T., Spering, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2007). Illusory bending of a rigidly moving line segment: Effects of image motion and smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision, 7(6):9, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/7/6/9/, doi:10.1167/7.6.9.

Thaler, V., Urton, K., Heine, A., Hawelka, S., Engl, V., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Different behavioral and eye movement patterns of dyslexic readers with and without attentional deficits during single word reading. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2436-2445.

Theeuwes, J., & Godijn, R. (2004). Inhibition of return and oculomotor interference. Vision Research, 44, 1485-1492.

Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition-of-return. Visual Cognition, 13, 657-665.

Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2009). Saccade trajectory deviations and inhibition-of-return: Measuring the amount of attentional processing. Vision Research, 49, 1307-1315.

Theeuwes, J., Belopolsky, A. V. (2012). Reward grabs the eye: Oculomotor capture by rewarding stimuli. Vision Research, 74, 80-85.

Theeuwes, J., Godijn R., & Pratt, J. (2004). A new estimation of the attentional dwell time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 60-64.

Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. E. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9, 379-385.

Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Irwin, D. E., & Zelinsky, G. J. (1999). Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, 1595-1608.

Theeuwes, J., Mathôt, S., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Object-based eye movements: the eyes prefer to stay within the same object. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 597-601.

Theeuwes, J., Olivers, C. N. L., & Chizk, C. L. (2005). Remembering a location makes the eyes curve away. Psychological Science, 16, 196-199.

Theeuwes, J., Van der Stigchel, S., & Olivers, C. N. L. (2006). Spatial working memory and inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 608-613.

Theeuwes, J., de Vries, G. J., & Godijn, R. (2003). Attentional and oculomotor capture with static singletons. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 735-746.

Thevarajah, D., Mikulic, A., & Dorris, M. C. (2009). Role of the superior colliculus in choosing mixed-strategy saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1998-2008.

Thevarajah, D., Webb, R., Ferrall, C., & Dorris, M. C. (2009). Modeling the value of strategic actions in the Superior Colliculus. Frontiers in Behavorial Neuroscience, 3:57. doi:10.3389/neuro.08.057.2009.

Theys, T., Pani, P., van Loon, J., Goffin, J., & Janssen, P. (2013). Three-dimensional shape coding in grasping circuits: a comparison between the anterior intraparietal area and ventral premotor area F5a. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 352-364.

Theys, T., Srivastava, S., van Loon, J., Goffin, J., & Janssen, P. (2012). Selectivity for three-dimensional contours and surfaces in the anterior intraparietal area. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107, 995-1008.

Thiel, M., Romano, M. C., Kurths, J., Rolfs, M., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Generating surrogates from recurrences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences, 366, 545-557.

Thiem, P. D. , Hill, J. A. , Lee, K.-M., & Keller, E. L. (2008). Behavioral properties of saccades generated as a choice response. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 355-364.

Thomas, C. L., Goegan, L. D., Newman, K. R., Arndt, J. E., & Sears, C. R. (2013). Attention to threat images in individuals with clinical and subthreshold symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27, 447-455.

Thomas, L. E. (2013). Spatial working memory is necessary for actions to guide thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1974-1981.

Thomas, L. E., & Irwin, D. E. (2006). Voluntary eyeblinks disrupt iconic memory. Perception and Psychophysics, 68, 475-488.

Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2007). Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 663-668.

Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009). Covert shifts of attention function as an implicit aid to insight. Cognition, 111, 168-174.

Thomas, M. G., Crosier, M., Lindsay, S., Kumar, A., Thomas, S., Araki, M., Talbot, C. J., McLean, R. J., Surendran, M., Taylor, K., Leroy, B. P., Moore, A. T., Hunter, D. G., Hertle, R. W., Tarpey, P. T., Langmann, A., Lindner, S., Brandner, M., & Gottlob, I. (2011). The clinical and molecular genetic features of idiopathic infantile periodic alternating nystagmus. Brain, 134, 892 - 902.

Thomas, M. G., Gottlob, I., McLean, R. J., Maconachie, G., Kumar, A., & Proudlock, F. A. (2011). Reading strategies in infantile nystagmus syndrome. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 52, 8156-8165.

Thomas, N. W., & Paré, M. (2007). Temporal processing of saccade targets in parietal cortex area LIP during visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 942-947.

Thomas, S., Critchley, P., Lawden, M., Farooq, S., Thomas, A., Proudlock, F. A., Constantinescu, S. C., & Gottlob, I. (2005). Stiff person syndrome with eye movement abnormality, myasthenia gravis, and thymoma. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76, 141-142.

Thomas, S., Proudlock, F. A., Sarvananthan, N., Roberts, E. O., Awan, M., McLean, R., Surendran, M., Kumar, A. S. A., Farooq, S. J., Degg, C., Gale, R. P., Reinecke, R. D., Woodruff, G., Langmann, A., Lindner, S., Jain, S., Tarpey, P., Raymond, F. L., & Gottlob, I. (2008). Phenotypical characteristics of idiopathic infantile nystagmus with and without mutations in FRMD7. Brain, 131, 1259-1267.

Thompson, A. A., & Henriques, D. Y. P. (2008). Updating visual memory across eye movements for ocular and arm motor control. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 2507-2514.

Thompson, A. A., & Henriques, D. Y. P. (2010). Locations of serial reach targets are coded in multiple reference frames. Vision Research, 50, 2651-2660.

Thompson, A. A., & Westwood, D. A. (2007). The hand knows something that the eye does not: Reaching movements resist the Müller-Lyer illusion whether or not the target is foveated. Neuroscience Letters, 416, 111-116.

Thompson, A. A., Glover, C. V., & Henriques, D. Y. P. (2012). Allocentrically implied target locations are updated in an eye-centred reference frame. Neuroscience Letters, 514, 214-218.

Thurtell, M. J., Dell'Osso, L. F., Leigh, R. J., Matta, M., Jacobs, J. B., & Tomsak, R. L. (2010). Effects of Acetazolamide on Infantile Nystagmus Syndrome Waveforms: Comparisons to Contact Lenses and Convergence in a Well-Studied Subject. The Open Ophthalmology Journal, 4, 42-51.

Tilikete, C., Koene, A., Nighoghossian, N., Vighetto, A., & Pélisson, D. (2006). Saccadic lateropulsion in Wallenberg syndrome: a window to access cerebellar control of saccades? Experimental Brain Research, 174, 555-565.

Titone, D., Libben, M., Mercier, J., Whitford, V., & Pivneva, I., (2011). Bilingual lexical access during L1 sentence reading: The effects of L2 knowledge, semantic constraint, and L1-L2 intermixing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1412-1431.

Todd, R. M., Talmi, D., Schmitz, T. W., Susskind, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Psychophysical and neural evidence for emotion-enhanced perceptual vividness. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 11201-11212.

Tong, J., Zhang, Z.-L., Cantor, C. R. L., & Schor, C. M. (2012). The effect of perceptual grouping on perisaccadic spatial distortions. Journal of Vision, 12(10):10, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/10/10, doi:10.1167/12.10.10.

Torab, K., Davis, T. S., Warren, D. J., House, P. A., Normann, R. A., & Greger, B. (2011). Multiple factors may influence the performance of a visual prosthesis based on intracortical microstimulation: nonhuman primate behavioural experimentation. Journal of Neural Engineering, 8, 035001 doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/3/035001.

Toscani M, Valsecchi M, Gegenfurtner KR. (2013). Selection of visual information for lightness judgements by eye movements. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130056. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0056.

Toscani, M., Valsecchi, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Optimal sampling of visual information for lightness judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 11163-11168;

Toscano, J. C., & McMurray, B. (2012). Cue-integration and context effects in speech: Evidence against speaking-rate normalization. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1284-1301.

Toscano, J. C., Anderson, N. D., & McMurray, B. (2013). Reconsidering the role of temporal order in spoken word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 981-987.

Towal, R.B., Mormann, M., & Koch, C. (2013). Simultaneous modeling of visual saliency and value computation improves predictions of economic choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, E3858-E3867.

Townsend, D. J. (2013). Aspectual coercion in eye movements. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 281-306.

Trauzettel-Klosinski, S. (2005). AMD-READ project: Assessment and optimisation of macular function with special regard to reading and motor control: An overview. International Congress Series, 1282, 741-744.

Tremblay, A. (2011). Learning to parse liaison-initial words: An eye-tracking study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 257-279.

Tremblay, A., & Spinelli, E. (2013). Segmenting liaison-initial words: The role of predictive dependencies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1093-1113.

Tremblay, C., Champoux, F., Voss, P., Bacon, B. A., Lepore, F., & Théoret, H. (2007). Speech and non-speech audio-visual illusions: A developmental study. PLoS ONE 2(8): e742. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000742.

Tremblay, S., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2009). Evidence of anticipatory eye movements in the spatial hebb repetition effect: Insights for modeling sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1256-1265.

Tremblay, S., Saint-Aubin, J., & Jalbert, A. (2006). Rehearsal in serial memory for visual-spatial information: Evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 452-457.

Trick, L., Hogarth, L., & Duka, T. (2011). Prediction and uncertainty in human Pavlovian to instrumental transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 757-765.

Tronconso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2005). Novel visual illusions related to Vasarely's 'nested squares' show that corner salience varies with corner angle. Perception, 34, 409-420.

Tronconso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in. Journal of Vision, 8(14):15, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/15/, doi:10.1167/8.14.15.

Tronconso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2009). Corner salience varies linearly with corner angle during flicker-augmented contrast: a general principle of corner perception based on Vasarely's artworks. Spatial Vision, 22, 211-224.

Tronconso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., Otero-Millan, G., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Microsaccades drive illusory motion in the Enigma illusion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 16033-16038.

Trottier, L., & Pratt, J. (2005). Visual processing of targets can reduce saccadic latencies. Vision Research, 45, 1349-1354.

Trude, A. M., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012). Talker-specific perceptual adaptation during online speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 979-1001.

Trude, A. M., Tremblay, A., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2013). Limitations on adaptation to foreign accents. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 349-367.

Trukenbrod, H. A., & Engbert, R. (2007). Oculomotor control in a sequential search task. Vision Research, 47, 2426-2443.

Trukenbrod, H. A., & Engbert, R. (2012). Eye movements in a sequential scanning task: Evidence for distributed processing. Journal of Vision, 12(1):5, 1-12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/1/5, doi:10.1167/12.1.5.

Tsai, J.-L. (2001). A multichannel PC tachistoscope with high resolution and fast display change capability. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 33, 524-531.

Tsai, J.-L., & McConkie, G. W. (2003). Where do Chinese readers send their eyes. In J. Hyönä, R. Radach & H. Deubel (Eds), The Mind's Eyes: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research (pp. 159-176). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Tsai, J.-L., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2012). Parafoveal semantic information extraction in traditional Chinese reading. Acta Psychologica, 141, 17-23.

Tsai, J.-L., Lee, C.-Y., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Yen, N.-S. (2004). Use of phonological codes for Chinese characters: Evidence from processing of parafoveal preview when reading sentences. Brain and Language, 91, 235-244.

Tsai, Y.-F., Viirre, E., Strychacz, C., Chase, B., & Jung, T.-P. (2007). Task performance and eye activity: Predicting behavior relating to cognitive workload. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 78, B176-185.

Tsang, C., & Chambers, C. G. (2011). Appearances aren't everything: Shape classifiers and referential processing in Cantonese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1065-1080.

Tsang, Y.-K., & Chen, H.-C. (2010). Morphemic ambiguity resolution in Chinese: Activation of the subordinate meaning with a prior dominant-biased context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 875-881.

Tse, P. U. (2005). Voluntary attention modulates the brightness of overlapping transparent surfaces. Vision Research, 45, 1095-1098.

Tse, P. U., & Hsieh, P.-J. (2006). The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals. Vision Research, 46, 3881-3885.

Tse, P. U., & Hsieh, P.-J. (2007). Component and intrinsic motion integrate in 'dancing bar' illusion. Biological Cybernetics, 96, 1-8.

Tse, P. U., Caplovitz, G. P., & Hsieh, P. -J. (2009). Corrigendum to "Microsaccade directions do not predict directionality of illusory brightness changes of overlapping transparent surfaces" [Vision Research 46 (2006) 3823-3830]". Vision Research, 49, 790.e1-790.e7.

Tse, P. U., Caplovitz, G. P., & Hsieh, P.-J. (2006). Microsaccade directions do not predict directionality of illusory brightness changes of overlapping transparent surfaces. Vision Research, 46, 3823-3830.

Tseng, F.-Y., Chao, C.-J., Feng, W.-Y., & Hwang, S.-L. (2013). Effects of display modality on critical battlefield e-map search performance. Behaviour & Information Technology, 32, 888-901.

Tseng, P.-H., Carmi, R., Cameron, I. G. M., Munoz, D. P., & Itti, L. (2009). Quantifying center bias of observers in free viewing of dynamic natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 9(7):4, 1-16, http://journalofvision.org/9/7/4/, doi:10.1167/9.7.4.

Tseng, Y.-C., & Li, R. C.-S. (2004). Oculomotor correlates of context-guided learning in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1363-1378.

Tseng, Y.-C., & Li, R. C.-S. (2008). The effects of response readiness and error monitoring on saccade countermanding. The Open Psychology Journal, 1, 18-25.

Tsuchiya, N., Moradi, F., Felsen, C., Yamazaki, M., & Adolphs, R. (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1224-1225.

Tsui, J. M. G., & Pack, C. C. (2011). Contrast sensitivity of MT receptive field centers and surrounds. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 1888-1900.

Tsui, J. M. G., Hunter, J. N., Born, R. T., & Pack, C. C. (2010). The role of V1 surround suppression in MT motion integration. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 3123-3138.

Tuinman, A., Mitterer, H., & Cutler, A. (2012). Resolving ambiguity in familiar and unfamiliar casual speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 530-544.

Turatto, M., & Betta, E. (2006). Redundant visual signals boost saccade execution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 928-932.

Turatto, M., Valsecchi, M., Tame, L., & Betta, E. (2007). Microsaccades distinguish between global and local visual processing. NeuroReport, 18, 1015-1018.

Turatto, M., Vescovi, M., & Valsecchi, M. (2007). Attention makes moving objects be perceived to move faster. Vision Research, 47, 166-178.

Turi, M., & Burr, D. (2012). Spatiotopic perceptual maps in humans: evidence from motion adaptation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 3091-3097.

Turk-Browne, N., & Pratt, J. (2005). Attending to eye movements and retinal eccentricity: Evidence for the Activity Distribution Model of Attention reconsidered. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1061-1066.

Back To Top

U

Uchida, Y., Kudoh, D., Higuchi, T., Honda, M., & Kanosue, K. (2013). Dynamic visual acuity in baseball players is due to superior tracking abilities. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 45, 319-325.

Uchida, Y., Kudoh, D., Murakami, A., Honda, M., & Kitazawa, S. (2012). Origins of superior dynamic visual acuity in baseball players: Superior eye movements or superior image processing. PLoS ONE 7(2): e31530. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031530.

Ueda, H., Takahashi, K., & Watanabe, K. (2013). Contributions of retinal input and phenomenal representation of a fixation object to the saccadic gap effect. Vision Research, 82, 52-57.

Ueda, Y., & Komiya, A. (2012). Cultural adaptation of visual attention: Calibration of the oculomotor control system in accordance with cultural scenes. PLoS ONE 7(11): e50282. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050282.

Ueda, Y., & Saiki, J. (2012) Characteristics of eye movements in 3-D object learning: Comparison between within-modal and cross-modal object recognition. Perception, 41, 1289-1298.

Underwood, G., & Foulsham, T. (2006). Visual saliency and semantic incongruency influence eye movements when inspecting pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59A, 1931-1949.

Underwood, G., Crundall, D., & Hodson, K. (2005). Confirming statements about pictures of natural scenes: Evidence of the processing of gist from eye movements. Perception, 34, 1069-1082.

Underwood, G., Foulsham, T., & Humphrey, K. (2009). Saliency and scan patterns in the inspection of real-world scenes: Eye movements during encoding and recognition. Visual Cognition, 17, 812-834.

Underwood, G., Foulsham, T., van Loon, E., & Underwood, J. (2005). Visual attention, visual saliency, and eye movements during the inspection of natural scenes. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 3562, 459-468.

Underwood, G., Foulsham, T., van Loon, E., Humphreys, L., & Bloyce, J. (2006). Eye movements during scene inspection: A test of the saliency map hypothesis. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 321-342.

Underwood, G., Humphrey, K., & van Loon, E. (2011). Decisions about objects in real-world scenes are influenced by visual saliency before and during their inspection. Vision Research, 51, 2031-2038.

Underwood, G., Jebbett, L., & Roberts, K. (2004). Inspecting pictures for information to verify a sentence; Eye movements in general encoding and in focused search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 165-182.

Underwood, G., Mennie, N., Humphrey, K., & Underwood, J. (2008). Remembering pictures of real-world images using eye fixation sequences in imagery and in recognition. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5329, 51-64.

Underwood, G., Phelps, N., Wright, C., van Loon, E., & Galpin, A. (2005). Eye fixation scanpaths of younger and older drivers in a hazard perception task. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 25, 346-356.

Underwood, G., Templeman, E., Lamming, L., & Foulsham, T. (2008). Is attention necessary for object identification? Evidence from eye movements during the inspection of real-world scenes. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 159-170.

Underwood, J., Templeman, E., & Underwood, G. (2009). Conspicuity and congruity in change detection. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5395, 85-97.

Unema, P. J. A., Pannasch, S., Joos, M., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2005). Time course of information processing during scene perception: The relationship between saccade amplitude and fixation duration. Visual Cognition, 12, 473-494.

Uppal, G., Feely, M., Crossland, M., Membrey, L., Lee, J., da Cruz, L., & Rubin, G. S. (2011). Assessment of reading behaviour with an infrared eyetracker following 360 degree macular translocation for age related macular degeneration. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 52, 6486-6496.

Upton, N. J., Hodgson, T. L., Plant, G. T., Wise, R. J. S., & Leff, A. P. (2003). "Bottom-up" and "top-down" effects on reading saccades: a case study. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74, 1423-1428.

Uzzaman, S., & Joordens, S. (2011). The eyes know what you are thinking: Eye movements as an objective measure of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1882-1886.

Back To Top

V

Vaden, R. J., Hutcheson, N. L., McCollum, L. A., Kentros, J., & Visscher, K. M. (2012). Older adults, unlike younger adults, do not modulate alpha power to suppress irrelevant information. Neuroimage, 63, 1127-1133.

Vainio, S., Bertram, R., Pajunen, A., & Hyönä, J. (2011). Processing modifier-head agreement in long Finnish words: Evidence from eye movements. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 58, 134-156.

Vainio, S., Hyönä, J., & Pajunen, A. (2003). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of grammatical agreement: Evidence from readers' eye fixation patterns. Brain and Language, 85, 197-202.

Vainio, S., Hyönä, J., & Pajunen, A. (2008). Processing modifier-head agreement in reading: Evidence for a delayed effect of agreement. Memory & Cognition, 36, 329-340.

Vainio, S., Hyönä, J., & Pajunen, A. (2009). Lexical predictability exerts robust effects on fixation duration, but not on initial landing position during reading. Experimental Psychology, 56, 66-74.

Valle, A., Binder, K. S., Walsh, C. B., Nemier, C., & Bangs, K. E. (2013). Eye movements, prosody, and word frequency among average- and high- skilled second-grade readers. School Psychology Review, 42, 171-190.

Valsecchi, M., & Turatto, M. (2007). Microsaccadic response to visual events that are invisible to the superior colliculus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 786-793.

Valsecchi, M., & Turatto, M. (2009). Microsaccadic responses in a bimodal oddball task. Psychological Research, 73, 23-33.

Valsecchi, M., Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2007). Visual oddballs induce prolonged microsaccadic inhibition. Experimental Brain Research, 177, 196-208.

Valsecchi, M., Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Schutz, A. C. (2013). Saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movements during reading of drifting texts. Journal of Vision, 13(10):8, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/10/8, doi:10.1167/13.10.8.

Valsecchi, M.,Toscani, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Perceived numerosity is reduced in peripheral vision. Journal of Vision, 13(13):7, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/contents/13/13/7, doi:10.1167/13.13.7.

Valuch, C., Becker, S. I., & Ansorge, U. (2013). Priming of fixations during recognition of natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 13(3):3, 1-22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/3, doi:10.1167/13.3.3.

Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., Duyck, W., Welvaert, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2011). The influence of semantic constraints on bilingual word recognition during sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 88-107.

Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Verb processing by bilinguals in sentence contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 237-259.

Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Diependaele, K. (2009). Does bilingualism change native-language reading? Cognate effects in a sentence context. Psychological Science, 20, 923-927.

Van Beers, R. J. (2007). The sources of variability in saccadic eye movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8757-8770.

van Beilen, M., Renken, R., Groenewold, E. S., & Cornelissen, F. W. (2011). Attentional window set by expected relevance of environmental signals. PLoS ONE 6(6): e21262. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021262.

Van Belle, G., Busigny, T., Lefèvre, P., Joubert, S., Felician, O., Gentile, F., & Rossion, B. (2011). Impairment of holistic face perception following right occipito-temporal damage in prosopagnosia: converging evidence from gaze-contingency. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3145-3150.

Van Belle, G., De Graef, P., Verfaillie, K., Rossion, B., & Lefèvre, P. (2010). Face inversion impairs holistic perception: Evidence from gaze-contingent stimulation. Journal of Vision, 10(5):10, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/content/10/5/10, doi:10.1167/10.5.10.

Van Belle, G., Lefèvre, P., Laguesse, R., Busigny, T., de Graef, P., Verfaillie, K., & Rossion, B. (2010). Feature-based processing of personally familiar faces in prosopagnosia: Evidence from eye-gaze contingency. Behavioural Neurology, 23, 255-257.

Van Belle, G., Ramon, M., Lefèvre, P., & Rossion, B. (2010). Fixation patterns during recognition of personally familiar and unfamiliar faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 1:20. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00020.

Van Belle, G., de Graef, P., Verfaillie, K., Busigny, T., & Rossion, B. (2010). Whole not hole: expert face recognition requires holistic perception. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2620-2629.

van Bochove, M. E., Van der Haegen, L., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2013). Blinking predicts enhanced cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 346-354.

Van Boxtel, J. J. A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2010). Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 8883-8888.

Van Dam, L. C. J., & Van Ee, R. (2005). The role of (micro)saccades and blinks in perceptual bi-stability from slant rivalry. Vision Research, 45, 2417-2435.

Van Dam, L. C. J., & van Ee, R. (2006). Retinal image shifts, but not eye movements per se, cause alternations in awareness during binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 6, 1172-1179.

Van Dam, L. C. J., & van Ee, R. (2006). The role of saccades in exerting voluntary control in perceptual and binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 46, 787-799.

van de Nieuwenhuijzen, M. E., Backus, A. R., Bahramisharif, A., Doeller, C. F., Jensen, O., & van Gerven, M. A. J. (2013). MEG-based decoding of the spatiotemporal dynamics of visual category perception. NeuroImage, 83, 1063-1073.

van de Velde, M., Meyer, A. S., & Konopka, A. E. (2014). Message formulation and structural assembly: Describing "easy" and "hard" events with preferred and dispreferred syntactic structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 124-144.

Van Dyke, J. A. (2007). Interference effects from grammatically unavailable constituents during sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 407-430.

Van Dyke, J. A., & McElree, B. (2011). Cue-dependent interference in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 247-263.

Van Ee, R., Noest, A. J., Brascamp, J. W., & van den Berg, A. V. (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts of ambiguous stimuli revealed by a two-parameter analysis: Means do not make the difference. Vision Research, 46, 3129-3141.

Van Ettinger-Veenstra, H. M., Huijbers, W., Gutteling, T. P., Vink, M., Kenemans, J. L., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2009). fMRI guided TMS on cortical eye fields: the frontal but not intraparietal eye fields regulate the coupling between visuo-spatial attention and eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 3469-3480.

Van Hagen, J. M., van der Geest, J. N., van der Giessen, R. S., Lagers-van Haselen, G. C., Eussen, H. J. F. M. M., Gille, J. J. P., Govaerts, L. C. P., Wouters, C. H., de Coo, I. F. M., Hoogenraad, C. C., Koekkoek, S. K. E., Frens, M. A., van Camp, N., van der Linden, A., Jansweijer, M. C. E., Thorgeirsson, S. S., & De Zeeuwb, C. I. (2007). Contribution of CYLN2 and GTF2IRD1 to neurological and cognitive symptoms in Williams syndrome. Neurobiology of Disease, 26, 112-124.

Van Humbeeck, N., Schmitt, N., Hermens, F., Wagemans, J., & Ernst, U. A. (2013). The role of eye movements in a contour detection task. Journal of Vision, 13(14):5, 1-19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/14/5, doi:10.1167/13.14.5.

van Lamsweerde, A. E., & Beck, M. R. (2012). Attention shifts or volatile representations: What causes binding deficits in visual working memory? Visual Cognition, 20, 771-792.

van Loon, A. M., Knapen, T., Scholte, H. S., St. John-Saaltink, E., Donner, T. H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2013). GABA shapes the dynamics of bistable perception. Current Biology, 23, 823-827.

Van Loon, E. M., Khashawi, F., & Underwood, G. (2010). Visual strategies used for time-to-arrival judgments in driving. Perception, 39, 1216-1229.

Van Pelt, S., & Medendorp, W. P. (2007). Gaze-centered updating of remembered visual space during active whole-body translations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 1209-1220.

Van Pelt, S., & Medendorp, W. P. (2008). Updating target distance across eye movements in depth. Journal of Neurophysiology, 99, 2281-2290.

van Pelt, S., & Fries, P. (2013). Visual stimulus eccentricity affects human gamma peak frequency. NeuroImage, 78,439-447.

Van Rensburg, K. J., Taylor, A., & Hodgson, T. (2009). The effects of acute exercise on attentional bias towards smoking-related stimuli during temporary abstinence from smoking. Addiction, 104, 1910-1917.

van Rijn, H., Dalenberg, J. R., Borst, J. P., & Sprenger, S. A. (2012). Pupil dilation co-varies with memory strength of individual traces in a delayed response paired-associate task. PLoS ONE 7(12): e51134. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051134.

Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., & Quinlan, T. (2010). Reading during sentence composing and error correction: A multilevel analysis of the influences of task complexity. Reading and Writing, 23, 803-834.

Van Zoest, W., & Donk, M. (2005). The effects of salience on saccadic target selection. Visual Cognition, 12, 353-375.

Van Zoest, W., & Donk, M. (2006). Saccadic target selection as a function of time. Spatial Vision, 19, 61-76.

Van Zoest, W., & Donk, M. (2008). Goal-driven modulation as a function of time in saccadic target selection. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1553-1572.

Van Zoest, W., & Donk, M. (2010). Awareness of the saccade goal in oculomotor selection: Your eyes go before you know. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 861-871.

Van Zoest, W., & Hunt, A. R. (2011). Saccadic eye movements and perceptual judgments reveal a shared visual representation that is increasingly accurate over time. Vision Research, 51, 111-119.

Van Zoest, W., Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2004). The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in visual selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 746-759.

Van Zoest, W., Lleras, A., Kingstone, A., & Enns, J. T. (2007). In sight, out of mind: The role of eye movements in the rapid resumption of visual search. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 1204-127.

Van Zoest, W., Van der Stigchel, S., & Barton, J. J. S. (2008). Distractor effects on saccade trajectories: a comparison of prosaccades, antisaccades, and memory-guided saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 431-442.

van Zoest, W., Donk, M., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2012) Stimulus-salience and the time-course of saccade trajectory deviations. Journal of Vision, 12(8)16, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/8/16, doi:10.1167/12.8.16.

Van den Berg, A. V., Beintema, J.A., & Frens, M. A. (2001). Heading and path percepts from visual flow and eye pursuit signals. Vision Research, 41, 3467-3486.

Van den Berg, R., Roerdink, J. B. T. M., & Cornelissen, F. W. (2007). On the generality of crowding: Visual crowding in size, saturation, and hue compared to orientation. Journal of Vision, 7(2):14, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/7/2/14/, doi:10.1167/7.2.14.

Van der Geest, J. N., & Frens, M. A. (2002). Recording eye movements with video-oculography and scleral search coils: a direct comparison of two methods. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 114, 185-195.

Van der Geest, J. N., Haselen, G. C. L.-N., & Frens, M. A. (2006). Saccade adaptation in Williams-Beuren syndrome. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 47, 1464-1468.

Van der Geest, J. N., Lagers-van Haselen, G. C., van Hagen, J. M., Govaerts, L. C. P., de Coo, I. F. M., de Zeeuw, C. I., & Frens, M. A. (2004). Saccade dysmetria in Williams-Beuren syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 42, 569-576.

Van der Haegen, L., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). The mechanisms underlying the interhemispheric integration of information in foveal word recognition: Evidence for transcortical inhibition. Brain and Language, 118, 81-89.

Van der Haegen, L., Cai, Q., Seurinck, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Further fMRI validation of the visual half field technique as an indicator of language laterality: A large-group analysis. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2879-2888.

Van der Haegen, L., Cai, Q., Stevens, M. A., & Brysbaert, M. (2013) Interhemispheric communication influences reading behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1442-1452.

Van der Haegen, L., Drieghe, D., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). The split fovea theory and the Leicester critique: What do the data say? Neuropsychologia, 48, 96-106.

Van der Meulen, F. F., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2001). Eye movements during the production of nouns and pronouns. Memory & Cognition, 29, 512-521.

Van der Schoot, M., Arkema, A. H. B., Horsley, T. M., & van Lieshout, E .C. D. M. (2009). The consistency effect depends on markedness in less successful but not successful problem solvers: An eye movement study in primary school children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 58-66.

Van der Schoot, M., Vasbinder, A. L., Horsley, T. M., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2008). The role of two reading strategies in text comprehension: An eye fixation study in primary school children. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 203-223.

van der Schoot, M., Horsley, T. M., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2010). The effects of instruction on situation model construction: an eye fixation study on text comprehension in primary school children. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 30, 817-835.

van der Schoot, M., Reijntjes, A., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2012). How do children deal with inconsistencies in text? An eye fixation and self-paced reading study in good and poor reading comprehenders. Reading and Writing, 25, 1665-1690.

van der Schoot, M., Vasbinder, A. L., Horsley, T. M., Reijntjes, A., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2009). Lexical ambiguity resolution in good and poor comprehenders: An eye fixation and self-paced reading study in primary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 21-36.

Van der Stigchel, S. (2010). The search for oculomotor inhibition: Interactions with working memory. Experimental Psychology, 57, 429-435.

Van der Stigchel, S., & Nijboer, T. C. W. (2010). The imbalance of oculomotor capture in unilateral visual neglect. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 186-197.

Van der Stigchel, S., & Nijboer, T. C. W. (2013). How global is the global effect? The spatial characteristics of saccade averaging. Vision Research, 84, 6-15.

Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2005). The influence of attending to multiple locations on eye movements. Vision Research, 45, 1921-1927.

Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2006). Our eyes deviate away from a location where a distractor is expected to appear. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 338-349.

Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). The relationship between covert and overt attention in endogenous cueing. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 719-731.

Van der Stigchel, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Differences in distractor induced deviation between horizontal and vertical saccade trajectories. NeuroReport, 19, 251-254.

Van der Stigchel, S., Arend, I., van Koningsbruggen, M. G., & Rafal, R. D. (2010). Oculomotor integration in patients with a pulvinar lesion. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3497-3504.

Van der Stigchel, S., Bethlehem, R.A.I., Klein, B.P., Berendschot, T.T.J.M., Nijboer, T.C.W., & Dumoulin, S.O. (2013). Macular degeneration affects eye movement behavior during visual search. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:579. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00579.

Van der Stigchel, S., Heeman, J., & Nijboer, T. C. W. (2012). Averaging is not everything: the saccade global effect weakens with increasing stimulus size. Vision Research, 62, 108-115.

Van der Stigchel, S., Imants, P. & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2011). Positive affect increases cognitive control in the antisaccade task. Brain & Cognition, 75, 177-181.

Van der Stigchel, S., Meeter, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). The spatial coding of the inhibition evoked by distractors. Vision Research, 47, 210-218.

Van der Stigchel, S., Meeter, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). Top-down influences make saccades deviate away: The case of endogenous cues. Acta Psychologica, 125, 279-290.

Van der Stigchel, S., Merten, H., Meeter, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). The effects of a task-irrelevant visual event on spatial working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 1066-1071.

Van der Stigchel, S., Mills, M., & Dodd, M. D. (2010). Shift and deviate: Saccades reveal that shifts of covert attention evoked by trained spatial stimuli are obligatory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1244-1250.

Van der Stigchel, S., Mulckhuyse, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2009). Eye can't see it: The interference of subliminal distractors on saccade metrics. Vision Research, 49, 2104-2109.

Van der Stigchel, S., Nijboer, T. C. W., Bergsma, D. P., Abegg, M., & Barton, J. J. S. (2010). Anomalous global effects induced by "blind" distractors in visual hemifield defects. Brain and Cognition, 74, 66-73.

Van der Stigchel, S., Nijboer, T. C. W., Bergsma, D. P., Barton, J. J. S., & Paffen, C. L. E. (2012). Measuring palinopsia: Characteristics of a persevering visual sensation from cerebral pathology. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 316, 184-188.

Van der Stigchel, S., Nijboer, T. C. W., Bultitude, J. H., & Rafal, R. D. (2013). Delayed oculomotor inhibition in patients with lesions to the human frontal oculomotor cortex: Evidence from a study on saccade averaging. Brain and Cognition, 82, 192-200.

Van der Stigchel, S., Rafal, R. D., & Bultitude, J. H. (2013). Temporal dynamics of error correction in a double step task in patients with a lesion to the lateral intra-parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2988-2994.

Van der Stigchel, S., Reichenbach, R. C. L., Wester, A. J., & Nijboer, T. C. W. (2012). Antisaccade performance in Korsakoff patients reveals deficits in oculomotor inhibition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 876-886.

Van der Stigchel, S., Rommelse, N. N. J., Deijen, J. B., Geldof, C. J. A., Witlox, J., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). Oculomotor Capture in ADHD. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 535-549.

Van der Stigchel, S., de Vries, J. P., Bethlehem, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). A global effect of capture saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 210, 57-65.

Van der Stigchel, S., van Koningsbruggen, M., Nijboer, T. C, W., List, A., & Rafal, R. D. (2012). The role of the frontal eye fields in the oculomotor inhibition of reflexive saccades: Evidence from lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 50, 198-203.

Van der Stigchel, S., van Zoest, W., Theeuwes, J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2008). The influence of 'blind' distractors on eye movement trajectories in visual hemifield defects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2025-2036.

Van der Stoep, N., Nijboer, T. C. W., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2012). Non-lateralized auditory input enhances averaged vectors in the oculomotor system. Experimental Brain Research, 221, 377-384.

Vancleef, K., Wagemans, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). Impaired texture segregation but spared contour integration following damage to right posterior parietal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 230, 41-57.

Vandierendonck, A., Deschuyteneer, M., Depoorter, A., & Drieghe, D. (2008). Input monitoring and response selection as components of executive control in pro-saccades and anti-saccades. Psychological Research, 72, 1-11.

Vangeneugden, J., De Mazière, P. A., Van Hulle, M. M., Jaeggli, T., Van Gool, L., & Vogels, L. (2011). Distinct mechanisms for coding of visual actions in macaque temporal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 385-401.

Vangeneugden, J., Pollick, F., & Vogels, R. (2009). Functional differentiation of macaque visual temporal cortical neurons using a parametric action space. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 593-611.

Vangeneugden, J., Vancleef, K., Jaeggli, T., VanGool, L., & Vogels, R. (2010). Discrimination of locomotion direction in impoverished displays of walkers by macaque monkeys. Journal of Vision, 10(4):22, 1-19, http://journalofvision.org/10/4/22/, doi:10.1167/10.4.22.

Vangkilde, S., & Habekost, T. (2010). Finding Wally: Prism adaptation improves visual search in chronic neglect. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1994-2004.

Vangkilde, S., Petersen, A., & Bundesen, C. (2013). Temporal expectancy in the context of a theory of visual attention. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130054.

Vanyukov, P. M., Warren, T., Wheeler, M. E., & Reichle, E. D. (2012). The emergence of frequency effects in eye movements. Cognition, 123, 185-189.

Varsori, M., Perez-Fornos, A., Safran, A. B., & Whatham, A. R. (2004). Development of a viewing strategy during adaptation to an artificial central scotoma. Vision Research, 44, 2691-2705.

Vasishth, S., & Drenhaus, H. (2011). Locality in German. Dialogue and Discourse, 1, 59-82.

Vasishth, S., Suckow, K., Lewis, R. L., & Kern, S. (2010). Short-term forgetting in sentence comprehension: Crosslinguistic evidence from verb-final structures. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 533-567.

Velan, H., Deutsch, A., & Frost, R. (2013). The flexibility of letter-position flexibility: Evidence from eye movements in reading Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 1143-1152.

Velichkovsky, B. M., Dornhoefer, S. M., Kopf, M., Helmert, J., & Joos, M. (2002). Change detection and occlusion modes in road-traffic scenarios. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 5, 99-109.

Velichkovsky, B. M., Rothert, A., Kopf, M., Dornhöfer, S. M., & Joos, M. (2002). Towards an express-diagnostics for level of processing and hazard perception. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 5, 145-156.

Verghese, P. (2012). Active search for multiple targets is inefficient. Vision Research, 74, 61-71.

Vergilino-Perez, D., Fayel, A., Lemoine, C., Senot, P., Vergne, J., & Dore-Mazars, K. (2012). Are there any left-right asymmetries in saccade parameters? Examination of latency, gain and peak velocity. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 53, 3340-3348.

Verhoef, B.-E., Vogels, R., & Janssen, P. (2011). Synchronization between the end-stages of the dorsal and the ventral visual stream. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 2030-2042.

Verhoef, B.-E., Vogels, R., & Janssen, P. (2012). Inferotemporal cortex subserves three-dimensional structure categorization. Neuron, 73, 171-182.

Verleger, R., Sprenger, A., Gebauer, S., Fritzmannova, M., Friedrich, M., Kraft, S., & Jaskowski, P. (2009). On why left events are the right ones: Neural mechanisms underlying the left-hemifield advantage in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 474-488.

Vermeiren, A. Liefooghe, B., & Vandierendonck, A. (2010). Switch performance in peripherally and centrally triggered saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 206, 243-248.

Vernet, M., & Kapoula, Z. (2009). Binocular motor coordination during saccades and fixations while reading: A magnitude and time analysis. Journal of Vision, 9(7):2, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/9/7/2/, doi:10.1167/9.7.2.

Vernet, M., Yang, Q., & Kapoula, Z. (2011). Guiding binocular saccades during reading: a TMS study of the PPC. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 5:14. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00014.

Vernet, M., Yang, Q., Daunys, G., Orssaud, C., & Kapoula, Z. (2008). Divergence influences triggering of both vertical and horizontal saccades. Optometry & Vision Science, 85, 187-195.

Vernet, M., Yang, Q., Daunys, G., Orssaud, C., & Kapoula, Z. (2008). TMS of the posterior parietal cortex delays the latency of unpredictable saccades but not when they are combined with predictable divergence. Brain Research Bulletin, 76, 50-56.

Vernet, M., Yang, Q., Daunys, G., Orssaud, C., Eggert, T., & Kapoula, Z. (2008). How the brain obeys Hering's Law: A TMS study of the posterior parietal cortex. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 49, 230-237.

Vernet, M., Yang, Q., Gruselle, M., Trams, M., & Kapoula, Z. (2009). Switching between gap and overlap pro-saccades: cost or benefit? Experimental Brain Research, 197, 49-58.

Verrel, J., Bekkering, H., & Steenbergen, B. (2008). Eye-hand coordination during manual object transport with the affected and less affected hand in adolescents with hemiparetic cerebral palsy. Experimental Brain Research, 187, 107-116.

Vesia, M., Prime, S. L., Yan, X., Sergio, L. E., & Crawford, J. D. (2010). Specificity of human parietal saccade and reach regions during transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 13053-13065.

Vetter, P., Edwards, G., & Muckli, L. (2012). Transfer of predictive signals across saccades. Frontiers in Psychology, 3: 176. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00176.

Vidal-Abarca, E., Martinez, T., Salmerón, L., Cerdán, R., Gilabert, R., Gil, L., Mañá, A., Llorens, A. C., & Ferris, R. (2011). Recording online processes in task-oriented reading with Read&Answer. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 179-192.

Vidoni, E. D., McCarley, J. S., Edwards, J. D., & Boyd, L. A. (2009). Manual and oculomotor performance develop contemporaneously but independently during continuous tracking. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 611-620.

Vig, E., Dorr, M., & Barth, E. (2009). Efficient visual coding and the predictability of eye movements on natural movies. Spatial Vision, 22, 397-408.

Vig, E., Dorr, M., Martinetz, T., & Barth, E. (2011). Eye movements show optimal average anticipation with natural dynamic scenes. Cognitive Computation, 3, 79-88.

Vigneau, F., Caissie, A. F., & Bors, D. A. (2006). Eye-movement analysis demonstrates strategic influences on intelligence. Intelligence, 34, 261-272.

Vigo, R., Zeigler, D. E., & Halsey, P. A. (2013). Gaze and informativeness during category learning: Evidence for an inverse relation. Visual Cognition, 21, 446-476.

Vilariño, F., Lacey, G., Zhou, J., Mulcahy, H., & Patchett, S. (2007). Automatic labeling of colonoscopy video for cancer detection. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4477, 290-297.

Vincent, B. T. (2012). How do we use the past to predict the future in oculomotor search? Vision Research, 74, 93-101.

Vincent, B. T., Correani, A., Baddeley, R. J., Troscianko, T., & Leonards, U. (2009). Do we look at lights? Using mixture modelling to distinguish between low- and high-level factors in natural image viewing. Visual Cognition, 17, 856-879.

Vinski, M.T., & Watter, S. (2013). Being a grump only makes things worse: a transactional account of acute stress on mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:730. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00730.

Visser, R.M., Scholte, H.S., Beemsterboer, T., & Kindt, M. (2013). Neural pattern similarity predicts long-term fear memory. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 388-390.

Viswanathan, J., & Barton, J. J. S. (2013). The global effect for antisaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 247-259.

Vlaskamp, B. N. S., & Hooge, I. Th. C. (2006). Crowding degrades saccadic search performance. Vision Research, 46, 417-425.

Vlaskamp, B. N. S., & Schubö, A. (2012). Eye movements during action preparation. Experimental Brain Research, 216, 463-472.

Vlaskamp, B. N. S., Over, E. A. B., & Hooge, I. Th. (2005). Saccadic search performance: The effect of element spacing. Experimental Brain Research, 167, 246-259.

von der Gablentz, J., Sprenger, A., Heldmann, M., Münte, T. F., & Helmchen, C. (2013). Acquired pendular nystagmus and its therapy in progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) due to inferior olivary hypertrophy. Journal of Neurology, 260, 2424-2426

von der Malsburg, T., & Vasishth, S. (2013). Scanpaths reveal syntactic underspecification and reanalysis strategies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1545-1578.

Von Grünau, M. W., Pilgrim, K., & Zhou, R. (2007). Velocity discrimination thresholds for flowfield motions with moving observers. Vision Research, 47, 2453-2464.

von Muhlenen, A., Watson, D., & Gunnell, D. O. (2013). Blink and you won't miss it: The preview benefit in visual marking survives internally generated eyeblinks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 1279-1290.

von Stockhausen, L., Koeser, S., Sczesny, S. (2013). The gender typicality of faces and its impact on visual processing and on hiring decisions. Experimental Psychology, 60, 444-452.

Von Wartburg, R., Ouerhani, N., Pflugshaupt, T., Nyffeler, T., Wurtz, P., Hugli, H., & Muri, R. M. (2005). The influence of colour on oculomotor behaviour during image perception. NeuroReport, 16, 1557-1560.

Von Wartburg, R., Wurtz, P., Pflugshaupt, T., Nyffeler, T., Lüthi, M., & Müri, R. M. (2007). Size matters: Saccades during scene perception. Perception, 36, 355-365.

Vorstius, C., Radach, R., & Lang, A. R. (2012). Effects of acute alcohol intoxication on automated processing: evidence from the double-step paradigm. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 26, 262 - 272.

Vorstius, C., Radach, R., Lang, A. R., & Riccardi, C. J. (2008). Specific visuomotor deficits due to alcohol intoxication: Evidence from the pro- and antisaccade paradigms. Psychopharmacology, 196, 201-210.

Vorstius, C., Radach, R., Mayer, M. B., & Lonigan, C. J. (2013). Monitoring local comprehension monitoring in sentence reading. School Psychology Review , 42, 191-206.

Voss, H. U., McCandliss, B. D., Ghajar, J., & Suh, M. (2007). A quantitative synchronization model for smooth pursuit target tracking. Biological Cybernetics, 96, 309-322.

Võ, M. L.-H., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Does gravity matter? Effects of semantic and syntactic inconsistencies on the allocation of attention during scene perception. Journal of Vision, 9(3):24, 1-15, http://journalofvision.org/9/3/24/, doi:10.1167/9.3.24.

Võ, M. L.-H., & Henderson, J. M. (2010). The time course of initial scene processing for eye movement guidance in natural scene search. Journal of Vision, 10(3):14, 1-13, http://journalofvision.org/10/3/14/, doi:10.1167/10.3.14.

Võ, M. L.-H., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). Object-scene inconsistencies do not capture gaze: evidence from the flash-preview moving-window paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1742-1753.

Võ, M. L.-H., & Schneider, W. X. (2010). A glimpse is not a glimpse: Differential processing of flashed scene previews leads to differential target search benefits. Visual Cognition, 18, 171-200.

Võ, M. L.-H., & Wolfe, J. M. (2012). When does repeated search in scenes involve memory? Looking at versus looking for objects in scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 23-41.

Võ, M. L.-H., & Wolfe, J. M. (2013). The interplay of episodic and semantic memory in guiding repeated search in scenes. Cognition, 126, 198-212.

Võ, M. L.-H., Schneider, W. X., & Matthias, E. (2008). Transsaccadic scene memory revisited: A 'Theory of Visual Attention (TVA)' based approach to recognition memory and confidence for objects in naturalistic scenes. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(2):7, 1-13.

Võ, M. L.-H., Zwickel, J., & Schneider, W. X. (2010). Has someone moved my plate? The immediate and persistent effects of object location changes on gaze allocation during natural scene viewing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1251-1255.

Back To Top

W

Wade, N. J., & Tatler, B. W. (2010). Recognition and eye movements with partially hidden pictures of faces and cars in different orientations. i-Perception, 1, 103-121.

Wagner, M., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Papathomas, T. V. (2009). Vergence in reverspective: Percept-driven versus data-driven eye movement control. Neuroscience Letters, 449, 142-146.

Walker, R. (2013). An iPad app as a low-vision aid for people with macular disease. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 97, 110-112.

Walker, R. M., McSorley, E., & Haggard, P. (2006). The control of saccade trajectories: direction of curvature depends upon prior knowledge of target location and saccade latency. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 129-138.

Walker, R., & Benson, V. (2013). Remote distractor effects and saccadic inhibition: Spatial and temporal modulation. Journal of Vision, 13(11):9, 1-21, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/11/9, doi:10.1167/13.11.9.

Walker, R., & McSorley, E. (2006). The parallel programming of voluntary and reflexive saccades. Vision Research, 46, 2082-2093.

Walker, R., & McSorley, E. (2008). The influence of distractors on saccade-target selection: Saccade trajectory effects. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(3):7, 1-13.

Walker, R., Techawachirakul, P., & Haggard, P. (2009). Frontal eye field stimulation modulates the balance of salience between target and distractors. Brain Research, 1270, 54-63.

Wallace, J. M., Chiu, M. K., Nandy, A. S., & Tjan, B. S. (2013). Crowding during restricted and free viewing. Vision Research, 84, 50-59.

Wallis, G. (2005). A spatial explanation for synchrony biases in perceptual grouping: Consequences for the temporal-binding hypothesis. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 345-353.

Wallis, G. (2006). The temporal and spatial limits of compensation for fixational eye movements. Vision Research, 46, 2848-2858.

Walthew, C., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2006). Target location probability effects in visual search: An effect of sequential dependencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 1294-1301.

Walton, M. M., Ono, S., & Mustari, M. J. (2013). Stimulation of pontine reticular formation in monkeys with strabismus. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54, 7125-7136.

Wang, C.-A., & Inhoff, A. W. (2010). The influence of visual contrast and case changes on parafoveal preview benefits during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 805-817.

Wang, C.-A., & Inhoff, A. W. (2013). Extraction of linguistic information from successive words during reading: Evidence for spatially distributed lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 662-677.

Wang, C.-A., Boehnke, S. E., White, B. J., & Munoz, D. P. (2012). Microstimulation of the Monkey Superior Colliculus Induces Pupil Dilation Without Evoking Saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 3629 - 3636.

Wang, C.-A., Inhoff, A. W., & Radach, R. (2009). Is attention confined to one word at a time? The spatial distribution of parafoveal preview benefits during reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1487-1494.

Wang, C.-A., Tsai, J.-L., Inhoff, A. W., & Tzeng, O. J.-L. (2009). Acquisition of linguistic information to the left of fixation during the reading of Chinese text. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 1097-1123.

Wang, H. X., Freeman, J., Merriam, E. P., Hasson, U., & Heeger, D. J. (2012). Temporal eye movement strategies during naturalistic viewing. Journal of Vision, 12(1):16, 1-27, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/1/16,doi:10.1167/12.1.16.

Wang, H.-C., & Pomplun, M. (2012). The attraction of visual attention to texts in real-world scenes. Journal of Vision, 12(6):26, 1-17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/6/26, doi:10.1167/12.6.26.

Wang, H.-C., Hwang, A. D., & Pomplun, M. (2010). Object Frequency and Predictability Effects on Eye Fixation Durations in Real-World Scene Viewing. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(3):3, 1-10.

Wang, H.-C., Pomplun, M., Chen, M., Ko, H., & Rayner, K. (2010). Estimating the effect of word predictability on eye movements in Chinese reading using latent semantic analysis and transitional probability. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1374-1386.

Wang, H.-C., Schotter, E. R., Angele, B., Yang, J., Simovici, D., Pomplun, M., & Rayner, K. (2013). Using singular value decomposition to investigate degraded Chinese character recognition: evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 36, S35-S50.

Wang, J. T.-Y., Spezio, M., & Camerer, C. F. (2010). Pinocchio's pupil: Using eyetracking and pupil dilation to understand truth telling and deception in sender-receiver games. The American Economic Review, 100, 984-1007.

Wang, J., Tian, J., Wang, R., & Benson, V. (2013). Increased attentional focus modulates eye movements in a mixed antisaccade task for younger and older adults. PLoS ONE 8(4): e61566. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061566.

Wang, J., Xia, R., Zhang, M., & Pan, Y. (2012). Long term retention of saccadic adaptation is induced by a dark environmental context. Brain Research, 1489, 56-65.

Wang, J.-C., & Day, R.-F. (2007). The effects of attention inertia on advertisements on the WWW. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1390-1407.

Wang, M.-Y., Tang, D.-L., Kao, C.-T., & Sun, V. C. (2013). Banner evaluation predicted by eye tracking performance and the median thinking style. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8013, 129-138.

Wang, Q., Bolhuis, J., Rothkopf, C. A., Kolling, T., Knopf, M., & Triesch, J. (2012). Infants in control: Rapid anticipation of action outcomes in a gaze-contingent paradigm. PLoS ONE 7(2): e30884. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030884.

Wang, S., Chen, H.-C., Yang, J., & Mo, L. (2008). Immediacy of integration in discourse comprehension: Evidence from Chinese readers' eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 241-257.

Wang, S., Fukuchi, M., Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2012). Spatial attention is attracted in a sustained fashion toward singular points in the optic flow. PLoS ONE 7(8): e41040. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041040.

Wang, Y., Iliescu, B. F., Ma, J., Josic, K., & Dragoi, V. (2011). Adaptive changes in neuronal synchronization in Macaque V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 13204-13213.

Wang, Z. I., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2008). Tenotomy procedure alleviates the "slow to see" phenomenon in infantile nystagmus syndrome: Model prediction and patient data. Vision Research, 48, 1409-1419.

Wang, Z. I., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2009). Factors influencing pursuit ability in infantile nystagmus syndrome: Target timing and foveation capability. Vision Research, 42, 182-189.

Wang, Z. I., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2011). A unifying model-based hypothesis for the diverse waveforms of Infantile Nystagmus Syndrome. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 4(1):1, 1-18

Wang, Z., & Dell'Osso, L. F. (2007). Being "slow to see" is a dynamic visual function consequence of infantile nystagmus syndrome: Model predictions and patient data identify stimulus timing as its cause. Vision Research, 47, 1550-1560.

Wang, Z., & Klein, R. M. (2012). Focal spatial attention can eliminate inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 462-469.

Wang, Z., & Roe, A. W. (2010). Trial-to-trial noise cancellation of cortical field potentials in awake macaques by autoregression model with exogenous input (ARX). Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 194, 266-273.

Wang, Z., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Dissociable spatial and temporal effects of inhibition of return. PLoS ONE 7(8): e44290. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044290.

Wang, Z., Dell'Osso, L. F., Jacobs, J. B., Burnstine, R. A., & Tomsak, R. L. (2006). Effects of tenotomy on patients with infantile nystagmus syndrome: Foveation improvement over a broadened visual field. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 10, 552-560.

Wang, Z., Dell'Osso, L. F., Tomsak, R. L., & Jacobs, J. B. (2007). Combining recessions (nystagmus and strabismus) with tenotomy improved visual function and decreased oscillopsia and diplopia in acquired downbeat nystagmus and in horizontal infantile nystagmus syndrome. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 11, 135-141.

Wang, Z., Dell'Osso, L. F., Zhang, Z., Leigh, R. J., & Jacobs, J. B. (2006). Tenotomy does not affect saccadic velocities: Support for the "small-signal" gain hypothesis. Vision Research, 46, 2259-2267.

Wang, Z., Satel, J., Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2012). Averaging saccades are repelled by prior uninformative cues at both short and long intervals. Visual Cognition, 20, 825-847.

Warren, D. E., Thurtell, M. J., Carroll, J. N., & Wall, M. (2013). Perimetric evaluation of saccadic latency, saccadic accuracy, and visual threshold for peripheral visual stimuli in young compared with older adults. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54, 5778-5787.

Warren, P. A., Rushton, S. K., & Foulkes, A. J. (2012). Does optic flow parsing depend on prior estimation of heading? Journal of Vision, 12(11):8, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/11/8, doi:10.1167/12.11.8.

Warren, T., McConnell, K., & Rayner, K. (2008). Effects of context on eye movements when reading about possible and impossible events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1001-1010.

Warren, T., Reichle, E., & Patson, N. (2011). Lexical and post-lexical complexity effects on eye movements in reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 4(1):3, 1-10.

Warren, T., White, S. J., & Reichle, E. D. (2009). Investigating the causes of wrap-up effects: Evidence from eye movements and E-Z Reader. Cognition, 111, 132-137.

Wass, S. V., Smith, T. J., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Parsing eye-tracking data of variable quality to provide accurate fixation duration estimates in infants and adults. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 229-250.

Watanabe, K., & Yokoi, K. (2006). Object-based anisotropies in the flash-lag effect. Psychological Science, 17, 728-735.

Watanabe, K., & Yokoi, K. (2007). Object-based anisotropic mislocalization by retinotopic motion signals. Vision Research, 12, 1662-1667.

Watanabe, K., & Yokoi, K. (2008). Dynamic distortion of visual position representation around moving objects. Journal of Vision, 8(3):13, 1-11, http://journalofvision.org/8/3/13/, doi:10.1167/8.3.13.

Waterston, M. L., Pack, C. C. (2010). Improved discrimination of visual stimuli following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS ONE 5(4): e10354. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010354.

Watson, D. G., & Inglis, M. (2007). Eye movements and time-based selection: Where do the eyes go in preview search? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 852-857.

Watson, D. G., Maylor, E. A., & Bruce, L. A. M. (2007). The role of eye movements in subitizing and counting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1389-1399.

Watson, K. K., & Platt, M. L. (2012). Social signals in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Current Biology, 22, 2268-2273.

Watson, K. K., Ghodasra, J. H., & Platt, M. L. (2009). Serotonin transporter genotype modulates social reward and punishment in rhesus macaques. PLoS ONE 4(1): e4156. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004156

Watson, M. R., Brennan, A. A., Kingstone, A., & Enns, J. T. (2010). Looking versus seeing: Strategies alter eye movements during visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 543-549.

Watson, T. L., & Krekelberg, B. (2009). The relationship between saccadic suppression and perceptual stability. Current Biology, 12, 1040-1043.

Watson, T., & Krekelberg, B. (2011). An equivalent noise investigation of saccadic suppression. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 6535-6541.

Weaver, M. D., & Lauwereyns, J. (2011). Attentional capture and hold: the oculomotor correlates of the change detection advantage for faces. Psychological Research, 75, 10-23.

Weaver, M. D., Aronsen, D., & Lauwereyns, J. (2012). A short-lived face alert during inhibition of return. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 510-520.

Weaver, M. D., Lauwereyns, J., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). The effect of semantic information on saccade trajectory deviations. Vision Research, 51, 1124-1128.

Weaver, M. D., Phillips, J., & Lauwereyns, J. (2010). Semantic influences from a brief peripheral cue depend on task set. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1249-1255.

Weber, A., & Crocker, M. W. (2012). On the nature of semantic constraints on lexical access. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 195-214.

Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 1, 1-25.

Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M. (2006). Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49, 367-392.

Weber, A., Grice, M., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). The role of prosody in the interpretation of structural ambiguities: A study of anticipatory eye movements. Cognition, 99, B63-B72.

Weger, U. W., & Inhoff, A. W. (2006). Attention and eye movements in reading: Inhibition of return predicts the size of regressive saccades. Psychological Science, 17, 187-191.

Weger, U. W., & Inhoff, A. W. (2007). Long-range regressions to previously read words are guided by spatial and verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1293-1306.

Wei, W., Li, X., & Pollatsek, A. (2013). Word properties of a fixated region affect outgoing saccade length in Chinese reading. Vision Research, 80, 1-6.

Weingartner, K. M., & Myers, J. L. (2013). Effects of changes in narrative time on eye movements and recognition responses. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 283-298.

Welchman, A. E., Harris, J. M., & Brenner, E. (2009). Extra-retinal signals support the estimation of 3D motion. Vision Research, 49, 782-789.

Welham, A. K., & Wills, A. J. (2011). Unitization, similarity, and overt attention in categorization and exposure. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1518-1533.

Wengelin, Å., Torrance, M., Holmqvist, K., Simpson, S., Galbraith, D., Johansson, V., & Johansson, R. (2009). Combined eye-tracking and keystroke-logging methods for studying cognitive processes in text production. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 337-351.

Wertheim, A. H., Hooge, I. T. C., Krikke, K., & Johnson, A. (2006). How important is lateral masking in visual search? Experimental Brain Research, 170, 387-402.

Werthmann, J., Field, M., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2014). Attention bias for chocolate increases chocolate consumption - An attention bias modification study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45, 136-143.

Werthmann, J., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2013). Desire lies in the eyes: Attention bias for chocolate is related to craving and self-endorsed eating permission. Appetite, 70, 81-89.

Werthmann, J., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Jansen, A. (2011). Can(not) take my eyes off it: Attention bias for food in overweight participants. Health Psychology, 30, 561-569.

Werthmann, J., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Jansen, A. (2013). Attention bias for food is independent of restraint in healthy weight individuals: An eye tracking study. Eating Behaviors, 14, 397-400.

West, G. L., Al-Aidroos, N., & Pratt, J. (2013). Action video game experience affects oculomotor performance. Acta Psychologica, 142, 38-42.

West, G. L., Al-Aidroos, N., Susskind, J., & Pratt, J. (2011). Emotion and action: the effect of fear on saccadic performance. Experimental Brain Research, 209, 153-158.

West, G., Welsh, T., and Pratt, J. (2009). Saccadic trajectories receive online correction: Evidence for a feedback-based system of oculomotor control. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 117-126.

Wexler, M., & Ouarti, N. (2008). Depth affects where we look. Current Biology, 18, 1872-1876.

White, A. L., Rolfs, M., & Carrasco, M. (2013). Adaptive deployment of spatial and feature-based attention before saccades. Vision Research, 85, 26-35.

White, B. J., Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Kerzel, D. (2005). Effects of structured nontarget stimuli on saccadic latency. Journal of Neurophysiology, 93, 3214-3223.

White, B. J., Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). The spatio-temporal tuning of the mechanisms in the control of saccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 46, 3886-3897.

White, B. J., Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). Visually guided movements to color targets. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 110-126.

White, B. J., Stritzke, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Saccadic facilitation in natural backgrounds. Current Biology, 18, 124-128.

White, K. S., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E., & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Adults show less sensitivity to phonetic detail in unfamiliar words, too. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 362-378.

White, S. J., & Staub, A. (2012). The distribution of fixation durations during reading: Effects of stimulus quality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 603-617.

White, S. J., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2008). Semantic processing of previews within compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 34, 988-993.

White, S. J., Warren, T., & Reichle, E. D. (2011). Parafoveal preview during reading: Effects of sentence position. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1221-1238.

Whitford, V. & Titone, D. (2012). Second-language experience modulates first- and second-language word frequency effects: Evidence from eye movement measures of natural paragraph reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 73-80.

Whitford, V., O'Driscoll, G. A., Pack, C. C., Joober, R., Malla, A., & Titone, D. (2013). Reading impairments in Schizophrenia relate to individual differences in phonological processing and oculomotor control: Evidence from a gaze-contingent moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 57-75.

Wichmann, F. A., Drewes, J., Rosas, P., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Animal detection in natural scenes: Critical features revisited. Journal of Vision, 10(4):6, 1-27, http://journalofvision.org/10/4/6/, doi:10.1167/10.4.6.

Wiener, J. M., Hölscher, C., Büchner, S., & Konieczny, L. (2012). Gaze behaviour during space perception and spatial decision making. Psychological Research, 76, 713-729.

Wienrich, C., Hesse, U., & Müller-Plath, G. (2009). Eye movements and attention in visual feature search with graded target-distractor-similarity. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(1):4, 1-19.

Wierda, S. M., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Martens, S. (2012). Pupil dilation deconvolution reveals the dynamics of attention at high temporal resolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 8456-8460.

Wiese, E., Zwickel, J., & Müller, H. J. (2013). The importance of context information for the spatial specificity of gaze cueing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 967-982.

Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1739-1749.

Wilimzig, C., Tsuchiya, N., Fahle, M., Einhäuser, W., & Koch, C. (2008). Spatial attention increases performance but not subjective confidence in a discrimination task. Journal of Vision, 8(5):7, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/8/5/7/, doi:10.1167/8.5.7.

Wilkowski, B. M., Robinson, M. D., Gordon, R. D., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2007). Tracking the evil eye: Trait anger and selective attention within ambiguously hostile scenes. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 650-666.

Williams, C. C. (2010). Not all visual memories are created equal. Visual Cognition, 18, 201-228.

Williams, C. C., & Pollatsek, A. (2007). Searching for an O in an array of Cs: Eye movements track moment-to-moment processing in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 372-381.

Williams, C. C., Pollatsek, A., Cave, K. R., & Stroud, M. J. (2009). More than just finding color: Strategy in global visual search is shaped by learned target probabilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 688-699.

Williams, D. E., & Reingold, E. M. (2001). Preattentive guidance of eye movements during triple conjunction search tasks: The effects of feature discriminability and saccadic amplitude. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 476-488.

Williams, D. E., Reingold, E. M., Moscovitch, M., & Behrmann, M. (1997). Patterns of eye movements during parallel and serial visual search tasks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 151-164.

Williams, T. A., Porter, M. A., & Langdon, R. (2013). Viewing social scenes: A visual scan-path study comparing Fragile X Syndrome and Williams Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1880-1894.

Williamson, V. M., Hegarty, M., Deslongchamps, G., Williamson, K. C. III, & Shultz, M. J. (2013). Identifying Student Use of Ball-and-Stick Images versus Electrostatic Potential Map Images via Eye Tracking. Journal of Chemical Education, 90, 159-164.

Willmore, B .D. B., Prenger, R. J., & Gallant, J. L. (2010). Neural representation of natural images in visual area V2. Jource of Neuroscience, 30, 2102-2114.

Willmore, B. D. B., Mazer, J. A., & Gallant, J. L. (2011). Sparse coding in striate and extrastriate visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 2907-2919.

Wills, A. J., Lavric, A., Croft, G. S., & Hodgson, T. L. (2007). Predictive learning, prediction errors, and attention: Evidence from Event-related Potentials and Eye Tracking. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 843-854.

Wilmer, J. B., & Nakayama, K. (2007). Two distinct visual motion mechanisms for smooth pursuit: Evidence from individual differences. Neuron, 54, 987-1000.

Wilming, N., Betz, T., Kietzmann, T. C., König, P. (2011). Measures and limits of models of fixation selection. PLoS ONE 6(9): e24038. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024038.

Wilming, N., Harst, S., Schmidt, N., & König, P. (2013). Saccadic momentum and facilitation of return saccades contribute to an optimal foraging strategy. PLoS Comput Biol 9(1): e1002871. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002871.

Wilson, A., Wilson, A., ten Hove, M. W., Paré, M., & Munhall, K. G. (2008). Loss of central vision and audiovisual speech perception. Visual Impairment Research, 10, 23-34.

Wilson, C. E., Brock, J., & Palermo, R. (2010). Attention to social stimuli and facial identity recognition skills in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 1104-1115.

Wilson, C. E., Palermo, R., & Brock, J. (2012). Visual scan paths and recognition of facial identity in autism spectrum disorder and typical development. PLoS ONE 7(5): e37681. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037681.

Wilson, G. F., Caldwell, J. A., & Russell, C. A. (2007). Performance and psychophysiological measures of fatigue effects on aviation related tasks of varying difficulty. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 17, 219-247.

Winges, S. A., & Soechting, J. F. (2011). Spatial and temporal aspects of cognitive influences on smooth pursuit. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 27-36.

Winke, P. M. (2013). Effects of input enhancement on grammar learning and comprehension. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 323-352.

Winke, P., Gass, S., & Sydorenko, T. (2013). Factors Influencing the Use of Captions by Foreign Language Learners: An Eye-Tracking Study. The Modern Language Journal, 97, 254-275.

Winskel, H. (2009). Reading in Thai: the case of misaligned vowels. Reading and Writing, 22, 1-24.

Winskel, H. (2011). Orthographic and phonological parafoveal processing of consonants, vowels, and tones when reading Thai. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 739-759.

Winskel, H., & Perea, M. (2013). Consonant/vowel asymmetries in letter position coding during normal reading: Evidence from parafoveal previews in Thai. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 119-130.

Winskel, H., Perea, M., & Ratitamkul, T. (2012). On the flexibility of letter position coding during lexical processing: Evidence from eye movements when reading Thai. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 68-73.

Winskel, H., Radach, R., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2009). Eye movements when reading spaced and unspaced Thai and English: A comparison of Thai-English bilinguals and English monolinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 339-351.

Winterboer, A. K., Tietze, M. I., Wolters, M. K., & Moore, J. D. (2011). The user model-based summarize and refine approach improves information presentation in spoken dialog systems. Computer Speech & Language, 25, 175-191.

Wismeijer, D. A., & Erkelens, C. J. (2009). The effect of changing size on vergence is mediated by changing disparity. Journal of Vision, 9(13):12, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/9/13/12/, doi:10.1167/9.13.12.

Wismeijer, D. A., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Orientation of noisy texture affects saccade direction during free viewing. Vision Research, 58, 19-26.

Wismeijer, D. A., Erkelens, C. J., van Ee, R., & Wexler, M. (2010). Depth cue combination in spontaneous eye movements. Journal of Vision, 10(6):25, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/25, doi:10.1167/10.6.25.

Wismeijer, D. A., van Ee, R., & Erkelens, C. J. (2008). Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence. Experimental Brain Research, 184, 61-70.

Wissig, S. C., Patterson, C. A., & Kohn, A. (2013). Adaptation improves performance on a visual search task. Journal of Vision, 13(2):6, 1-15 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/2/6, doi:10.1167/13.2.6.

Wissmath, B., Stricker, D., Weibel, D., Siegenthaler, E., & Mast, F.W. (2010). The illusion of being located in dynamic virtual environments. Can eye movement parameters predict spatial presence ? Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(5):2, 1-8.

Witkowski, M., Neville, B., & Pitt, J. (2005). Agent mediated retailing in the connected local community. Interacting with Computers, 15, 5-32.

Wittenberg, M., Bremmer, F., & Wachtler, T. (2008). Perceptual evidence for saccadic updating of color stimuli. Journal of Vision, 8(14):9, 1-9, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/9/, doi:10.1167/8.14.9.

Witzel, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Is there a lateralized category effect for color? Journal of Vision, 11(12):16, 1-25, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/12/16, doi:10.1167/11.12.16.

Wochna, K. L., & Juhasz, B. J. (2013). Context length and reading novel words: An eye-movement investigation. British Journal of Psychology, 104, 347-363.

Wolohan, F. D. A., & Crawford, T. J. (2012). The anti-orienting phenomenon revisited: effects of gaze cues on antisaccade performance. Experimental Brain Research, 221, 385-392.

Wolohan, F. D. A., Bennett, S. J. V., & Crawford, T. J. (2013). Females and attention to eye gaze: effects of the menstrual cycle. Experimental Brain Research, 227, 379-386.

Woloszyn, L., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2009). Neural dynamics in inferior temporal cortex during a visual working memory task. Journal of Neuroscience,29, 5494-5507.

Woloszyn, L., & Sheinberg, D. L. (2012). Effects of long-term visual experience on responses of distinct classes of single units in inferior temporal cortex. Neuron, 74, 193-205.

Wolter, L., Gorman, K. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). Scalar reference, contrast and discourse: Separating effects of linguistic discourse from availability of the referent. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 299-317.

Wong, J. H., & Peterson, M. S. (2011). The interaction between memorized objects and abrupt onsets in oculomotor capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1768-1779.

Wong, J. H., & Peterson, M. S. (2013). What we remember affects how we see: Spatial working memory steers saccade programming. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 308-321.

Wong, J. H., Peterson, M. S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (2007). Are changes in semantic and structural information sufficient for oculomotor capture? Journal of Vision, 7(12):3, 1-10, http://journalofvision.org/7/12/3/, doi:10.1167/7.12.3.

Wong, K. F. E., & Chen, H.-C. (1999). Orthographic and phonological processing in reading Chinese text: Evidence from eye fixations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 461-480.

Wonnacott, E., Newport, E. L., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Acquiring and processing verb argument structure: Distributional learning in a miniature language. Cognitive Psychology, 56, 165-209.

Woodhead, Z. V. J., Brownsett, S. L. E., Dhanjal, N. S., Beckmann, C., & Wise, R. J. S. (2011). The visual word form system in context. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 193-199.

Woods, H.C., Scheepers, C., Ross, K.A., Espie, C.A., & Biello, S.M. (2013). What are you looking at? moving toward an attentional timeline in insomnia: a novel semantic eye tracking study. Sleep, 36, 1491-1499.

Wotschack, C., & Kliegl, R. (2013). Reading strategy modulates parafoveal-on-foveal effects in sentence reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 548-562.

Wright, J. M., Morris, A. P., & Krekelberg, B. (2011). Weighted integration of visual position information. Journal of Vision, 11(14):11, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/11, doi:10.1167/11.14.11.

Wright, T. J., Boot, W. R., & Morgan, C. S. (2013). Pupillary response predicts multiple object tracking load, error rate, and conscientiousness, but not inattentional blindness. Acta Psychologica, 144, 6-11.

Wu, C.-C., & Kowler, E. (2013). Timing of saccadic eye movements during visual search for multiple targets. Journal of Vision, 13(11):11, 1-21, http://www.journalofvision.org/contents/13/11/11, doi:10.1167/13.11.11.

Wu, C.-C., Kwon, O.-S., & Kowler, E. (2010). Fitts's Law and speed/accuracy trade-offs during sequences of saccades: Implications for strategies of saccadic planning. Vision Research, 50, 2142-2157.

Wu, D. H., Morganti, A., & Chatterjee, A. (2008). Neural substrates of processing path and manner information of a moving event. Neuropsychologia, 46, 704-713.

Wu, D.-A., Shimojo, S., Wang, S. W., & Camerer, C. F. (2012). Shared visual attention reduces hindsight bias. Psychological Science, 23, 1524-1533.

Wu, E. X. W., Gilani, S. O., van Boxtel, J. J. A., Amihai, I., Chua, F. K., & Yen, S.-C. (2013). Parallel programming of saccades during natural scene viewing: Evidence from eye movement positions. Journal of Vision, 13(12):17, 1-14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/12/17, doi:10.1167/13.12.17.

Wu, L., Pu, J., Allen, J. J. B., & Pauli, P. (2012). Recognition of facial expressions in individuals with elevated levels of depressive symptoms: An eye-movement study. Depression Research and Treatment, vol. 2012, Article ID 249030, 7 pages, 2012. doi:10.1155/2012/249030.

Wu, L.-D., Pu, J., Allen, J. J. B., & Pauli, P. (2012). Recognition of facial expressions in individuals with elevated levels of depressive symptoms: An eye-movement study. Depression Research and Treatment, vol. 2012, Article ID 249030, 7 pages, 2012. doi:10.1155/2012/249030.

Wu, Y.-J., Cristino, F., Leek, C., & Thierry, G. (2013). Non-selective lexical access in bilinguals is spontaneous and independent of input monitoring: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognition, 129, 418-425.

Wyble, B., Potter, M. C., & Mattar, M. (2012). RSVP in orbit: Identification of single and dual targets in motion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 553-562.

Wöstmann, N. M., Aichert, D. S., Costa, A., Rubia, K., Möller, H. J., & Ettinger, U. (2013). Reliability and plasticity of response inhibition and interference control. Brain & Cognition, 81, 82-94.

Back To Top

X

Xiao, L.-Q., Zhang, J.-Y., Wang, R., Klein, S. A., Levi, D. M., & Yu, C. (2008). Complete transfer of perceptual learning across retinal locations enabled by double training. Current Biology, 18, 1922-1926.

Xiao, M., Wong, M., Umali, M., & Pomplun, M. (2007). Using eye-tracking to study audio-visual perceptual integration. Perception, 36, 1391-1395.

Xie, C., Zhu, T., Guo, C., & Zhang, Y. (2013). Measuring IVIS impact to driver by on-road test and simulator experiment. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 96. 1566-1577.

Xu, B. & Tanaka, K.W. (2013). Does face inversion qualitatively change face processing: An eye movement study using a face change detection task. Journal of Vision, 13(2):22, 1-16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/2/22, doi:10.1167/13.2.22.

Xu, Y., & Franconeri, S. L. (2012). The Head of the Table: Marking the "Front" of An Object Is Tightly Linked with Selection The Journal of Neuroscience, 25 January 2012, 32(4):1408-1412;

Xu, Y., Higgins, E. C., Xiao, M., & Pomplun, M. (2007). Mapping the color space of saccadic selectivity in visual search. Cognitive Science, 31, 877-887.

Xu-Wilson, M., Tian, J., Shadmehr, R., & Zee, D. S. (2011). TMS perturbs saccade trajectories and unmasks an internal feedback controller for saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 11537-11546.

Back To Top

Y

Yamagishi, N., Anderson, S. J., & Kawato, M. (2010). The observant mind: self-awareness of attentional status. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, Series B., 277, 3421-3426.

Yan, G., Bai, X., Zang, C., Bian, Q., Cui, L., Qi, W., Rayner, K., & Liversedge, S. P. (2012). Using stroke removal to investigate Chinese character identification during reading: evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing, 25, 951-979.

Yan, G., Tian, H., Bai, X., & Rayner, K. (2006). The effect of word and character frequency on the eye movements of Chinese readers. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 259-268.

Yan, M., Kliegl, R., Richter, E., Nuthmann, A., & Shu, H. (2010). Flexible saccade-target selection in Chinese reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 705-725.

Yan, M., Kliegl, R., Shu, H., Pan, J., & Zhou, X. (2010). Parafoveal load of word n+1 modulates preprocessing effectiveness of word n+2 in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1669-1676.

Yan, M., Pan, J., Laubrock, J., Kliegl, R., & Shu, H. (2013). Parafoveal processing efficiency in rapid automatized naming: A comparison between Chinese normal and dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 579-589.

Yan, M., Richter, E., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2009). Chinese readers extract semantic information from parafoveal words during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 561-566.

Yan, M., Risse, S., Zhou, X., & Kliegl, R. (2012). Preview fixation duration modulates identical and semantic preview benefit in Chinese reading. Reading and Writing, 25, 1093-1111.

Yan, M., Zhou, W., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2012). Lexical and sublexical semantic preview benefits in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1069-1075.

Yang, H., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2009). Visual search is guided to categorically defined targets. Vision Research, 49, 2095-2103.

Yang, H., Chen, X., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2009). A new look at novelty effects: Guiding search away from old distractors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 554-564.

Yang, H., Wang, F., Gu, N., Gao, X., & Zhao, G. (2013). The cognitive advantage for one’s own name is not simply familiarity: An eye-tracking study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1176-1180.

Yang, J. (2013). Preview effects of plausibility and character order in reading Chinese transposed words: evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading, 36, S18-S34.

Yang, J., Rayner, K., Li, N., & Wang, S. (2012). Is preview benefit from word n + 2 a common effect in reading Chinese? Evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing, 25, 1079-1091.

Yang, J., Staub, A., Li, N., Wang, S., & Rayner, K. (2012). Plausibility effects when reading one- and two-Character words in Chinese: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1801-1809..

Yang, J., Wang, S., Chen, H.-C., & Rayner, K. (2009). The time course of semantic and syntactic processing in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1164-1176.

Yang, J., Wang, S., Tong, X., & Rayner, K. (2012). Semantic and plausibility effects on preview benefit during eye fixations in Chinese reading. Reading and Writing, 25, 1031-1052.

Yang, J., Wang, S., Xu, Y., & Rayner, K. (2009). Do Chinese readers obtain preview benefit from word n + 2? Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 1192-1204.

Yang, Q., & Kapoula, Z. (2011). Distinct control of initiation and metrics of memory-guided saccades and vergence by the FEF: A TMS study. PLoS ONE 6(5): e20322. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020322.

Yang, Q., Vernet, M., Orssaud, C., Bonfils, P., Londero, A., & Kapoula, Z. (2010). Central crosstalk for somatic Tinnitus: Abnormal vergence eye movements. PLoS. ONE 5(7): e11845. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011845.

Yang, S. N., Tai, Y. C., Sheedy, J. E., Kinoshita, B., Lampa, M., & Kern, J. R. (2012). Comparative effect of lens care solutions on blink rate, ocular discomfort and visual performance. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 32, 412-420.

Yang, S.-N. (2009). Effects of gaze-contingent text changes on fixation duration in reading. Vision Research, 49, 2843-2855.

Yang, S.-N., & McConkie, G. W. (2001). Eye movements during reading: A theory of saccade initiation times. Vision Research, 41, 3567-3585.

Yang, S.-N., & McConkie, G. W. (2004). Saccade generation during reading: Are words necessary? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 226-261.

Yang, S.-N., Tai, Y.-C., Laukkanen, H., & Sheedy, J. E. (2011). Effects of ocular transverse chromatic aberration on peripheral word identification. Vision Research, 51, 2273-2281.

Yang, Z., Jackson, T., & Chen, H. (2013). Effects of chronic pain and pain-related fear on orienting and maintenance of attention: An eye movement study. The Journal of Pain, 14, 1148-1157.

Yang, Z., Jackson, T., Gao, X., & Chen, H. (2012). Identifying selective visual attention biases related to fear of pain by tracking eye movements within a dot-probe paradigm. Pain, 153, 1742-1748.

Yantis, S., Schwarzbach, J., Serences, J. T., Carlson, R. L., Steinmetz, M. A., Pekar, J. J., & Courtney, S. M. (2002). Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shift. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 995-1002.

Yanulevskaya, V., Marsman, J., Cornelissen, F., & Geusebroek, J.-M. (2011). An image statistics-based model for fixation prediction. Cognitive Computation, 3, 94-104.

Yao, B., & Scheepers, C. (2011). Contextual modulation of reading rate for direct versus indirect speech quotations. Cognition, 121, 447-453.

Yao, J.-G., Gao, X., Yan, H.-M., & Li, C.-Y. (2011). Field of attention for instantaneous object recognition. PLoS ONE 6(1): e16343. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016343.

Yatabe, K., Pickering, M. J., & McDonald, S. A. (2009). Lexical processing during saccades in text comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 62-66.

Yates, M., Friend, J., & Ploetz, D. M. (2008). The effect of phonological neighborhood density on eye movements during reading. Cognition, 107, 685-692.

Yee, E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Eye movements to pictures reveal transient semantic activation during spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 1-14.

Yee, E., Blumstein, S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2008). Lexical-semantic activation in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 592-612.

Yee, E., Huffstetler, S., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2011). Function follows form: Activation of shape and function features during object identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 348-363.

Yee, E., Overton, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Looking for meaning: Eye movements are sensitive to overlapping semantic features, not association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 869-874.

Yeh, L.-H., Schwartz, A. I., & Baule, A. L. (2011). The impact of text-structure strategy instruction on the text recall and eye-movement patterns of second language English readers. Reading Psychology, 32, 495-519.

Yeh, S.-L., He, S., & Cavanagh, P. (2012). Semantic priming from crowded words. Psychological Science, 23, 608-616.

Yen, M.-H., Lee, C.-N., & Yang, Y.-C. (2012). Eye movement patterns in solving scientific graph problems. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 7352, 343-345.

Yen, M.-H., Radach, R., Tzeng, O. & Tsai, J.-L. (2012). Usage of statistical cues for word boundary in reading Chinese sentences. Reading & Writing, 25, 1007-1029.

Yen, M.-H., Radach, R., Tzeng, O. J.-L., Hung, D. L., & Tsai, J. L. (2009). Early parafoveal processing in reading Chinese sentences. Acta Psychologica, 131, 24-33.

Yen, M.-H., Tsai, J.-L., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Hung, D. L. (2008). Eye movements and parafoveal word processing in reading Chinese. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1033-1045.

Yeung, L.-K., Ryan, J. D., Cowell, R. A., & Barense, M. D. (2013).Recognition memory impairments caused by false recognition of novel objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1384-1397.

Yi, W., & Ballard, D. H. (2009). Recognizing behavior in hand-eye coordination patterns. International Journal of Humanoid Robotics, 6, 337-359. Doi:10.1142/S0219843609001863.

Yigit-Elliott, S., Palmer, J., & Moore, C. M. (2011). Distinguishing blocking from attenuation in visual selective attention. Psychological Science, 22, 771-780.

Yoder, K. J., & Belmonte, M. K. (2010). Combining computer game-based behavioural experiments with high-density EEG and infrared gaze tracking. Journal of Visualized Experiment, 46, https://www.jove.com/details.stp?id=2320, doi: 10.3791/2320.

Yokoi, K., Watanabe, K., & Saida, S. (2012). Rapid and implicit effects of color category on visual search. Optical Review, 19, 276-281.

Yokoyama, T., Noguchi, Y., & Kita, S. (2012). Attentional shifts by gaze direction in voluntary orienting: evidence from a microsaccade study. Experimental Brain Research, 223, 291-300.

Yoon, D., & Narayanan, N. H. (2004). Predictors of success in diagrammatic problem solving. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 2980, 301-315.

Yoon, S. O., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2013). Lexical differentiation in language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 397-416.

Yoshida, K., Saito, N., Iriki, A. & Isoda, M. (2012). Social error monitoring in macaque frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 1307-1312.

Young, A. H., & Hulleman, J. (2013). Eye movements reveal how task difficulty moulds visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 168-190.

Yu, C. W.-M., Petheram, K., Davidson, A. W., Williams, T., & Griffiths, P. G. (2011). A supranuclear disorder of ocular motility as a rare initial presentation of motor neurone disease. Neuro-Ophthalmology, 35, 38-39.

Yun, K., Peng, Y., Samaras, D., Zelinsky, G.J., & Berg, T.L. (2013). Exploring the role of gaze behavior and object detection in scene understanding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:917. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00917.

Yuval-Greenberg, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2011). Scalp-recorded induced gamma-band responses to auditory stimulation and its correlations with saccadic muscle-activity. Brain Topography, 24, 30-39.

Yuval-Greenberg, S., Tomer, O., Keren, A. S., Nelken, I., & Deouell, L. Y. (2008). Transient induced Gamma-band response in EEG as a manifestation of miniature saccades. Neuron, 58, 429-441.

Back To Top

Z

Zang, C., Liang, F., Bai, X., Yan, G., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Interword spacing and landing position effects during Chinese eeading in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 720-734.

Zanker, J. M., Doyle, M., & Walker, R. (2003). Gaze stability of observers watching Op Art pictures. Perception, 32, 1037-1049.

Zanos, T. P., Mineault, P. J., & Pack, C. C. (2011). Removal of spurious correlations between spikes and local field potentials. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105, 474-486.

Zapata, L. P., Aznar-Casanova, J. A., & Supèr, H. (2013). Two stages of programming eye gaze shifts in 3-D space. Vision Research, 86, 15-26.

Zehetleitner, M., Hegenloh, M., & Müller, H. J. (2011). Visually guided pointing movements are driven by the salience map. Journal of Vision, 11(1):24, 1-18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/1/24, doi:10.1167/11.1.24.

Zehetleitner, M., Koch, A.I., Goschy, H., & Müller, H.J. (2013). Salience-based selection: Attentional capture by distractors less salient than the target. PLoS ONE 8(1): e52595. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052595.

Zeki, S., & Stutters, J. (2013). Functional specialization and generalization for grouping of stimuli based on colour and motion. NeuroImage, 73, 156-166.

Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). A theory of eye movements during target acquisition. Psychological Review, 115, 787-835.

Zelinsky, G. J. (2012). TAM: Explaining off-object fixations and central fixation tendencies as effects of population averaging during search. Visual Cognition, 20, 515-545.

Zelinsky, G. J., & Neider, M. B. (2008). An eye movement analysis of multiple object tracking in a realistic environment. Visual Cognition, 16, 553-566.

Zelinsky, G. J., & Schmidt, J. (2009). An effect of referential scene constraint on search implies scene segmentation. Visual Cognition, 17, 1004-1028.

Zelinsky, G. J., & Todor, A. (2010). The role of "rescue saccades" in tracking objects through occlusions. Journal of Vision, 10(14):29, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/29, doi:10.1167/10.14.29.

Zelinsky, G. J., Adeli, H., Peng, Y., & Samaras, D. (2013). Modelling eye movements in a categorical search task. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130058. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0058.

Zelinsky, G. J., Peng, Y., & Samaras, D. (2013). Eye can read your mind: Decoding gaze fixations to reveal categorical search targets. Journal of Vision, 13(14):10, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/14/10, doi:10.1167/13.14.10.

Zelinsky, G. J., Peng, Y., Berg, A. C., & Samaras, D. (2013). Modeling guidance and recognition in categorical search: Bridging human and computer object detection. Journal of Vision, 13(3):30, 1-20, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/30, doi:10.1167/13.3.30.

Zerouali, Y., Lina, J.-M., & Jemel, B. (2013). Optimal eye-gaze fixation position for face-related neural responses. PLoS ONE 8(6): e60128. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0060128.

Zhang, E., & Li, W. (2010). Perceptual learning beyond retinotopic reference frame. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 15969-15974.

Zhang, E., Zhang, G.-L., & Li, W. (2013). Spatiotopic perceptual learning mediated by retinotopic processing and attentional remapping. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 3758-3767.

Zhang, H., Morvan, C., & Maloney, L. T. (2010). Gambling in the visual periphery: A conjoint-measurement analysis of human ability to judge visual uncertainty. PLoS Computational Biology, 6(12): e1001023. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001023.

Zhang, H., Morvan, C., Etezad-Heydari, L.-A., & Maloney, L. T. (2012). Very slow search and reach: Failure to maximize expected gain in an eye-hand coordination task. PLoS Comput Biol 8(10): e1002718. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002718.

Zhang, H., Yan, H.-M., Kendrick, K. M., & Li, C.-Y. (2012). Both lexical and non-lexical characters are processed during saccadic eye movements. PLoS ONE 7(9): e46383. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046383.

Zhang, J.-Y., Zhang, G.-L., Liu, L., & Yu, C. (2012). Whole report uncovers correctly identified but incorrectly placed target information under visual crowding. Journal of Vision, 12(7):5, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/7/5, doi:10.1167/12.7.5.

Zhang, L., Ren, J., Xu, L., Qiu, X. J., & Jonas, J. B. (2013). Visual comfort and fatigue when watching three-dimensional displays as measured by eye movement analysis. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 97, 941-942.

Zhang, L., Zhang, Y.-Q., Zhang, J.-S., Xu, L., & Jonas, J. B. (2013). Visual fatigue and discomfort after stereoscopic display viewing. Acta Ophthalmologica, 91, e149-e153.

Zhang, R., Kwon, O.-S., & Tadin, D. (2013). Illusory movement of stationary stimuli in the visual periphery: Evidence for a strong centrifugal prior in motion processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4415-4423.

Zhang, T., Xiao, L.-Q., Klein, S. A., Levi, D. M., & Yu, C. (2010). Decoupling location specificity from perceptual learning of orientation discrimination. Vision Research, 50, 368-374.

Zhang, Y., & Zhang, M. (2011). Spatial working memory load impairs manual but not saccadic inhibition of return. Vision Research, 51, 147-153.

Zhang, Z.-L., Cantor, C. R. L., & Schor, C. M, (2010). Perisaccadic stereo depth with zero retinal disparity. Current Biology, 20, 1176-1181.

Zhang, Z.-L., Cantor, C. R. L., & Schor, C. M. (2008). Effects of luminance and saccadic suppression on perisaccadic spatial distortions. Journal of Vision, 8(14):22, 1-18, http://journalofvision.org/8/14/22/, doi:10.1167/8.14.22.

Zhao, G., Liu, Q., Jiao, J., Zhou, P., Li, H., & Sun, H. (2012). Dual-state modulation of the contextual cueing effect: Evidence from eye movement recordings. Journal of Vision, 12(6):11, 1-13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/6/11, doi:10.1167/12.6.11.

Zhou, H., & Desimone, R. (2011). Feature-based attention in the frontal eye field and area V4 during visual search. Neuron, 70, 1205-1217.

Zhou, P., & Gao, L. (2009). Scope processing in Chinese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38, 11-24.

Zhou, P., Crain, S., & Zhan, L. (2012). Sometimes children are as good as adults: The pragmatic use of prosody in children’s on-line sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 149-164.

Zhou, P., Su, Y., Crain, S., Gao, L., & Zhan, L. (2012). Children's use of phonological information in ambiguity resolution: a view from Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 39, 687-730.

Zhou, W., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2013). A validation of parafoveal semantic information extraction in reading Chinese. Journal of Research in Reading, 36, S51-S63,

Zhou, Y., Liu, Y., Zhang, W., & Zhang, M. (2012). Asymmetric influence of egocentric representation onto allocentric perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 8354-8360.

Zhu, W., Drewes, J., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Animal detection in natural images: Effects of color and image database. PLoS ONE 8(10): e75816. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075816.

Zhu, X. L., Tan, S. P., Yang, F. D., Sun, W., Song, C. S., Cui, J. F., Zhao, Y. L., Fan, F. M., Li, Y. J., Tan, Y. L., & Zou, Y. Z. (2013). Visual scanning of emotional faces in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Letters, 552, 46-51.

Zimmermann, E. (2013). The reference frames in saccade adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 109, 1815-1823.

Zimmermann, E., & Lappe, M. (2009). Mislocalization of flashed and stationary visual stimuli after adaptation of reactive and scanning saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 11055-11064.

Zimmermann, E., & Lappe, M. (2010). Motor signals in visual localization. Journal of Vision, 10(6):2, 1-11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/2, doi:10.1167/10.6.2.

Zimmermann, E., Burr, D., & Morrone, M. C. (2011). Spatiotopic visual maps revealed by saccadic adaptation in humans. Current Biology, 21, 1380-1384.

Zimmermann, E., Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. (2012). Visual motion distorts visual and motor space. Journal of Vision, 12(2):10, 1-8, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/2/10, doi:10.1167/12.2.10.

Zimmermann, E., Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. C. (2013). Spatial position information accumulates steadily over time. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 18396-18401.

Zirnsak, M., Gerhards, R. G. K., Kiani, R., Lappe, M., & Hamker, F. H. (2011). Anticipatory saccade target processing and the presaccadic transfer of visual features. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 17887-17891.

Zoccolan, D., Cox, D. D., & DiCarlo, J. J. (2005). Multiple object response normalization in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 8150-8164.

Zou, H., Müller, H. J., & Shi, Z. (2012). Non-spatial sounds regulate eye movements and enhance visual search. Journal of Vision, 12(5):2, 1-18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/5/2, doi: 10.1167/12.5.2.

Zuiderbaan, W., Harvey, B. M., & Dumoulin, S. O. (2012). Modeling center-surround configurations in population receptive fields using fMRI. Journal of Vision, 12(3):10, 1-15, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/3/10, doi:10.1167/12.3.10.

Zwickel, J., & Müller, H. J. (2009). Eye movements as a means to evaluate and improve robots. International Journal of Social Robotic, 1, 357-366.

Zwickel, J., & Võ, M. L.-H. (2010). How the presence of persons biases eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 257-262.

Zwickel, J., Hegele, M., & Grosjean, M. (2012). Ocular tracking of biological and nonbiological motion: The effect of instructed agency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 52-57.

Zylberberg, A., Barttfeld, P., & Sigman, M. (2012). The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6:79. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00079.

Zylberberg, A., Oliva, M., & Sigman, M. (2012). Pupil dilation: a fingerprint of temporal selection during the "attentional blink". Frontiers in Psychology, 2012;3:316. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00316.